Many studies have compared convergence and divergence behaviors reporting differences between the systems. Divergence is in the opposite direction of convergence; yet, it is not merely negative convergence. Dynamic analyses have generally reported that convergence is faster than divergence in humans (Hung, Zhu, & Ciuffreda,
1997; Zee, Fitzgibbon, & Optican,
1992) and macaque monkeys (Maxwell & King,
1992). Unlike convergence, divergence dynamics are dependent on the initial position of the stimulus; hence, dynamic comparisons need to account for differences in initial position (Alvarez, Semmlow, & Pedrono,
2005). Using a gap paradigm, researchers report that divergence can demonstrate shorter latencies than nongap responses; however, convergence did not show temporal differences (Coubard, Daunys, & Kapoula,
2004). When investigating the adaptive affects of sustained near convergence, nonlinear differences in an adaptive mechanism are noted between convergence and divergence (Patel, Jiang, White, & Ogmen,
1999). Furthermore, it has been shown that convergence and divergence have different influences on saccadic movements during saccade–vergence interaction studies (Busettini & Mays,
2003). Vertical saccade–vergence interaction shows that convergence velocities do not typically vary but divergence is dependent on the upward or downward vertical saccadic movement (Kumar, Han, Dell'osso, Durand, & Leigh,
2005). Convergence and divergence also exhibit distinct dysfunctions (Schor,
1988). Neurophysiologists have shown different cells encode convergence and divergence (Gamlin, Yoon, & Zhang,
1996; Mays,
1984; Mays & Porter,
1984; Mays, Porter, & Tello, 1986; Nitta, Akao, Kurkin, & Fukushima,
2008; Zhang & Gamlin,
1998; Zhang, Gamlin, & Mays,
1991). Hence, differences between the behaviors of the two systems should be anticipated.