The interaction between pursuit and saccades starts on the level of the visual input. Saccades are mainly guided by position signals, whereas pursuit mostly relies on velocity signals. But there is also evidence for velocity input to the saccadic system for the control of catch-up saccades during pursuit (Blohm, Missal, & Lefèvre,
2005a; de Brouwer, Yuksel, Blohm, Missal, & Lefèvre,
2002), and for position input to the pursuit system (Blohm, Missal, & Lefèvre,
2005b; Missal, Lefèvre, Delinte, Crommelinck, & Roucoux,
1996). Pursuit and saccades differ substantially in latency. In humans, pursuit is usually initiated at a latency of about 100–150 ms, whereas saccades take considerably longer, about 200–250 ms (Krauzlis,
2004; Rashbass,
1961). However, the time to movement onset can be influenced in the same way and to the same extent by task and visual stimulus used. Pursuit and saccade latencies are reduced by a gap between fixation-point offset and target onset to a similar degree (gap paradigm; Krauzlis & Miles,
1996a,
1996b). When the target appears before the disappearance of the fixation point, pursuit latency is prolonged and pursuit and saccadic latencies are highly correlated (overlap paradigm; Erkelens,
2006). Pursuit and saccade tracking are also similarly influenced by visual spatial attention (Adler, Bala, & Krauzlis,
2002; Krauzlis, Zivotofsky, & Miles,
1999; Madelain, Krauzlis, & Wallman,
2005). Furthermore, pursuit and saccades are tightly coupled with regard to the selection of a visual target (Gardner & Lisberger,
2001; Liston & Krauzlis,
2003,
2005) and share processing at the level of response preparation (Joiner & Shelhamer,
2006). Evidence from neurophysiological studies in monkeys and functional imaging studies in humans supports these behavioral similarities in visual processing, target selection, and response preparation. Pursuit and saccades are processed in parallel cortical pathways. They share processing in premotor and motor areas (e.g., Dicke, Barash, Ilg, & Thier,
2004; Gardner & Lisberger,
2002; Keller & Missal,
2003; Krauzlis & Dill,
2002; Krauzlis & Miles,
1996b; Missal & Keller,
2002; for reviews, see Krauzlis,
2004,
2005) but are controlled by distinct subregions (Petit & Haxby,
1999; Rosano et al.,
2002).