Top-down attentional deployment and shifts of gaze are tightly linked (Bichot & Schall,
1999; Corbetta et al.,
1998; Moore & Armstrong,
2003; Rizzolatti, Riggio, Dascola, & Umilta,
1987; Schall & Hanes,
1993). Here, we used eye position (gaze) as an indirect measurement of where attention is currently deployed in space. However, we do not assume that during a fixation attention is restricted to the closest nearby item. In fact, the detection part of our model processes a number of elements (determined by
C) in parallel at every fixation, as long as they are within the radius of detection (
D parameter). Planning where to fixate next does assume that focal attention shifts to the new location. These assumptions are supported by a number of neurophysiological studies. In particular, neurons located in the FEF are known to be closely related to the initiation of eye movements (Bruce & Goldberg,
1985; Bruce et al.,
1985; Schall, Hanes, et al.,
1995). The response of FEF neurons is dominated by the visual input that is task relevant, whereas all other input is only weakly represented (regardless of their visual features). The firing rate of an FEF neuron is higher if the item in the RF shares features with the target compared to an item that shares no features with the target (Bichot & Schall,
1999). FEF neurons thus signal, for each item, the estimated probability that this position contains the target. Based on this, it has been proposed that FEF represents an integration of the visual input together with top-down information about the task (Thompson & Bichot,
2005; Thompson, Bichot, & Schall,
2001). Looking at our model, FEF neurons can be thought of as implementing our target map (
Figure 3). Thus, each value
λ(
x) in the target map corresponds to the mean firing rate of neurons coding for a particular movement vector (relative to the current fixation). Also, the process of making a saccade where
λ(
x) is maximal has a close neuronal analogy: FEF neurons integrate their input until a threshold is reached (race-to-threshold model; Hanes & Schall,
1996). Thus, the neuron with the largest
λ(
x) will (on average) reach threshold fastest and evoke a saccade.