Texture segregation is the effortless division of a visual stimulus into distinct segments based on spatial gradients in local feature properties. Psychophysical studies suggest that texture segregation can be performed preattentively (Julesz,
1981a,
1981b). On the other hand, attention is considered to be essential for controlling perceptual processing, for example, by biasing the competition among stimuli for neural representation (see reviews by Kastner & Pinsk,
2004; Kastner & Ungerleider,
2000; Reynolds & Chelazzi,
2004; Serences & Yantis,
2006).
Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) have been frequently used to investigate texture segregation in the human cortex. In these experiments, VEPs to both homogeneous stimuli (without global pattern) and segregated stimuli (where contrasts in local features form a global pattern) were recorded and the “texture segregation VEP” (tsVEP) was isolated by subtracting the two VEP curves (Bach & Meigen,
1990,
1992; Lamme, Van Dijk, & Spekreijse,
1992). The latencies reported for the main components of tsVEPs considerably differ within and between studies, from approximately 110 ms to more than 250 ms (e.g., Bach & Meigen,
1999; Caputo & Casco,
1999; Fahle, Quenzer, Braun, & Spang,
2003; Lachapelle, Ouimet, Bach, Ptito, & McKerral,
2004; Schubö, Meinecke, & Schröger,
2001).
It is known from VEPs evoked by simple stimuli, such as luminance-defined checkerboard patterns, that attention may affect early responses including the P100 (e.g., Di Russo & Spinelli,
1999; Hoshiyama & Kakigi,
2001). These early effects were usually found when subjects had to attend to a specific location in the visual field while feature attention was generally associated with later effects (Hillyard & Kutas,
1983). Caputo and Casco (
1999) report that an additional component occurs in the VEP when the subjects are performing a discrimination task on a global figure rather than viewing the stimuli passively. In a more recent study investigating the effect of attention on texture segregation, Casco, Grieco, Campana, Corvino, and Caputo (
2005) required the subjects either to attend the segregated figure and judge its orientation or to attend to centrally presented numbers. They found that the main tsVEP component spanning the time range of 130–220 ms was reduced by about 40% when the numbers were attended. Demonstrating the importance of attention in figure-ground processing, the study by Casco et al. prompts new questions:
-
Symmetric task. Casco et al. guided the subjects' attention by requiring them to perform a task on the segregated stimulus. This results in an unbalanced situation because the homogenous stimuli are completely irrelevant for the task although the associated VEPs are used for computing the tsVEP.
-
Underlying mechanisms. The reduction of segregation-related activity in the number task might be due to competition in visual processing. Such competition might be either a general mechanism to yield the most relevant visual information (cf. Kastner & Ungerleider,
2000) or a result of limited resources. Another possibility, however, is that attentional mechanisms act across sensory modalities. Directing attention to one modality might then result in a deactivation of the other modality, as demonstrated in imaging studies (e.g., Johnson & Zatorre,
2006; Kawashima, O'Sullivan, & Roland,
1995).
In a recent study, Scholte, Witteveen, Spekreijse, and Lamme (
2006) used an elaborate rapid sequential visual presentation paradigm. They asked subjects to perform a task on centrally presented letters that appeared together with homogenous textures or occasionally with segregated patterns. In a first run, subjects were unaware of the segregated patterns. In a second run, they were informed and then perceived the segregation. Although the earliest segregation-related components in the magnetoencephalogram occurred already at around 200 ms, differences between runs, that is, between seen and unseen segregation, were only found after 400 ms.
An event-related potentials study by Schubö et al. (
2001) specifically aimed at investigating two late segregation-related components. The first, a relative positivity, peaked around 270 ms, that is, substantially later than most segregation-related components in other studies, and did not occur with a distracting task. The second had its maximum parietally around 400 ms and was modulated by task complexity.
In fMRI experiments where subjects had to attend to centrally presented numbers, Kastner, De Weerd, and Ungerleider (
2000) and Schira, Fahle, Donner, Kraft, and Brandt (
2004) did not find a sizable activation in V1, as opposed to previous studies without a distracting task (Schmitt, Janz, Hennig, & Bach,
1998; Skiera, Petersen, Skalej, & Fahle,
2000). There is no consensus among studies whether conscious perception is necessary for activation of area V3A (Kastner et al.,
2000; Schira et al.,
2004; Scholte et al.,
2006).
The question whether task-related attention affects a perceptual process is closely related to the question to what degree top-down processes are involved. Such attention-related feedback to the visual cortex might originate from frontal and parietal cortex (Kastner & Ungerleider,
2000; Serences & Yantis,
2006). Specifically for texture segregation, masking experiments in monkeys indicate that feedback at least from extrastriate visual areas to V1 exists (Lamme, Zipser, & Spekreijse,
2002). Task-related response modulations would imply that some feedback streams indeed originate from beyond the visual cortex. The finding that sufficiently deep anesthesia abolishes segregation responses in monkey V1 while leaving the classical receptive field properties unaffected also hints toward higher processing levels being involved in texture segregation (Lamme, Zipser, & Spekreijse,
1998).
We pursued the questions of top-down control and the attention-related modulation of visual processing across sensory modalities through comparison of various visual tasks and an auditory task. In the present study, this auditory task exclusively aimed at distracting the subjects from the visual stimuli. We therefore expected the effect to be of a general modulatory nature. This is different from those experiments that investigated how auditory stimuli alter the perception of visual stimuli in a very specific manner. Such findings have been recently reported by a number of authors. For instance, Shams, Kamitani, Thompson, and Shimojo (
2001) and Shams, Iwaki, Chawla, and Bhattacharya (
2005) have shown that auditory stimulation is able to trigger additional visual percepts that were not physically present and evoke responses in the visual cortex. Another example is a study by Beer and Röder (
2005), which demonstrated that attention to a specific direction of auditory motion altered the detection of that direction in visual motion. This direction-specific psychophysical effect was paralleled by changes in the VEP.