Orienting is generally done overtly by moving the eyes, head, and body (Land,
2004; Land & Hayhoe,
2001). With regard to saccadic eye movements, it has been firmly established that abrupt onsets can capture the eyes under a range of conditions (Boot, Kramer, & Peterson,
2005; Godijn & Theeuwes,
2002; Irwin, Colcombe, Kramer, & Hahn,
2000; Ludwig & Gilchrist,
2002,
2003a; Theeuwes et al.,
1998; Theeuwes, Kramer, Hahn, Irwin, & Zelinsky,
1999; Wu & Remington,
2003). A number of these studies have attempted to compare oculomotor capture by abrupt onsets with other visual events. Wu and Remington (
2003) showed that, as with covert attention, oculomotor capture was influenced by the search mode or set adopted by observers. However, capture by abrupt onsets was never quite eliminated in conditions in which the contingent capture view predicts it should not occur. Irwin et al. (
2000) compared the extent of oculomotor capture by abrupt onsets with that elicited by transient color and luminance changes. They reported that abrupt onsets were especially effective. Finally, Boot et al. (
2005) compared capture by abrupt onsets and offsets and showed that the disappearance of an item did not elicit an inappropriate saccade even though the two events are similar in terms of the magnitude of sensory change. Thus, there is some evidence that abrupt onsets have special status where the eye movement system is concerned. Indeed, within oculomotor research a prevalent assumption is that the saccadic system is highly sensitive to rapid changes in luminance (Lennie,
1993).