When steadily fixating a target in space we usually perceive the position of the target as stationary. However, under such steady fixation, the eye is not physically stationary but exhibits small eye movements (Ditchburn,
1973; Steinman,
1976) so that the retinal image of the “steadily” fixated target constantly traverses the retina in a manner determined by the pattern of eye movements that comprise the fixation jitter. It has been postulated that the functional significance of such retinal jitter is to overcome the fading of retinal images that would occur if retinal images were physically stationary on the retina (Riggs, Ratliff, Cornsweet, & Cornsweet,
1953). If this retinal image jitter is considered as motion noise, one might predict that in order to discriminate the true motion of a target, the true target motion must be greater than the spurious motion produced by retinal jitter (Murakami,
2004). If so, minimum motion thresholds should be far greater than 1 arcmin because the components of eye movements comprising fixation eye movements include fixation tremors (<1 arcmin), microsaccades (typically around 5 to 10 arcmin), and drifts (≅10 arcmin) (Ditchburn & Foley-Fischer,
1967; Eizenman, Hallett, & Frecker,
1985; Ratliff & Riggs,
1950). However, since all targets in the field share this eye motion generated component of motion, a relative motion judgment might be much more sensitive.
The human visual system is indeed extremely sensitive at making judgments of relative motion. Under optimal stimulus conditions subjects are able to discriminate target motions smaller than the diameter of a foveal cone photoreceptor (Legge & Campbell,
1981; Nakayama & Tyler,
1981; Westheimer,
1978). In order to achieve this high degree of motion sensitivity, the visual system must employ mechanisms that can differentiate real target motion from spurious motion due to retinal jitter. Murakami (
2003,
2004) and Murakami and Cavanagh (
1998) showed that in the absence of a reliable reference, the visual system is unable to effectively compensate for retinal motion. Physically stationary targets then appear to move. Furthermore, Murakami (
2004) showed that eye velocity was correlated with motion thresholds if the surround was absent or flickered. The results of Tulunay-Keesey and VerHoeve (
1987) also lend support for the idea that spatial references play a key role in the compensation for fixation eye movements. They showed that motion thresholds for an oscillating line are significantly elevated if the background against which motion judgments are made was stabilized. Furthermore, both studies showed that minimum motion thresholds were consistently higher for conditions without a spatial reference than for the conditions with a spatial reference. Similar findings of lower minimum motion thresholds with referenced versus unreferenced conditions were reported by Legge and Campbell (
1981), Levi, Klein, and Aitsebaomo (
1984), Shioiri, Ito, Sakurai, and Yaguchi (
2002), and Whitaker and MacVeigh (
1990). These studies collectively provide strong evidence that the presence of spatial references engages mechanisms that compensate for the effects of spurious image motion produced by retinal jitter and also allude to the deleterious effects of fixation eye movements on unreferenced motion thresholds.
Previous attempts to study the relationship between motion thresholds and fixation eye movements (Murakami,
2004; Tulunay-Keesey & VerHoeve,
1987) employed eye movement tracking devices to infer retinal movement. However, many of these instruments are capable of resolving eye movements with an optimal precision of about an arcmin (Stevenson & Roorda,
2005), and their accuracy depends on the subject's fixation during calibrations. This level of resolution imposes an obvious restriction when studying the effect of small eye movements on motion thresholds, which are often smaller than 1 arcmin. In the present experiment we employed high-resolution retinal imaging using the adaptive optics scanning laser ophthalmoscope (AOSLO) (Roorda et al.,
2002). The AOSLO has two advantages. The first advantage is its capacity to produce high-resolution retinal imaging at the level of a cone photoreceptor. The second advantage is that the AOSLO imaging beam can be modulated to present a psychophysical stimulus (Poonja, Patel, Henry, & Roorda,
2005). The combined effect of these two advantages was that we were able to stimulate and image the retina simultaneously, thereby providing an extremely sensitive method by which to study the effects of fixation jitter on motion judgments.
In the present study we show that referenced motion thresholds are unaffected by retinal jitter due to fixation eye movements. This independence between referenced motion thresholds and retinal jitter is produced by compensatory mechanisms that are largely visually driven and require the presence of a spatial reference. Unreferenced motion thresholds are adversely affected by retinal jitter, being higher for larger magnitudes of retinal jitter due to fixation eye movements and strongly biased by the overall drift of the eye. We also conclude that compensatory processes in unreferenced motion may be partial and speculate that only certain types of eye movements are being compensated and not others.