There has been a lot of debate as to whether or not humans can actually solve the eye-rotation problem in the context of a moving observer (e.g., Li, Sweet, & Stone,
2006; Li & Warren,
2000; Royden et al.,
1992; Stone & Perrone,
1997; van den Berg,
1993; Warren & Hannon,
1988; Wilkie & Wann,
2006). Some initial studies showed that heading could be extracted reasonably accurately from motion flow fields without the aid of an extraretinal signal (Warren & Hannon,
1988), but later studies showed that when the speed of pursuit eye movements was higher, an extraretinal signal is important for correct performance. It is now reasonably well established that an ‘extraretinal’ (eye movement) signal does help us visually navigate and that some cancellation process is at work (Crowell & Andersen,
2001; Freeman, Banks, & Crowell,
2000; Li & Warren,
2000; Royden et al.,
1992). The fact that we can navigate safely while making eye movements indicates that at some stage the brain must be compensating for the eye movements. Neural correlates of a form of cancellation have been demonstrated in neurons found in the Medial Superior Temporal (MSTd) area of the primate brain (Bradley, Maxwell, Andersen, Banks, & Shenoy,
1996.; Erikson & Thier,
1991; Inaba, Shinomoto, Yamane, Takemura, & Kawano,
2007; Lee, Pesaran, & Andersen,
2007; Page & Duffy,
1999; Shenoy, Bradley, & Andersen,
1999; Upadhyay, Page, & Duffy,
2000) as well as in the parietal area VIP (Zhang, Heuer, & Britten,
2004). There is also clinical evidence that the ability to compensate for eye movements is lost when cortical lesions are present (Haarmeier, Thier, Repnow, & Petersen,
1997). We will therefore assume that some form of correction occurs but acknowledge that it may not always work perfectly (Crowell & Andersen,
2001; Freeman & Banks,
1998; Freeman et al.,
2000; Haarmeier, Bunjes, Lindner, Berret, & Thier,
2001; van den Berg, Beintema, & Frens,
2001).