The slant thresholds for each subject are provided in
Figure 5. We used a maximum likelihood approach to compute weighted group means based on the
SEMs of the individual data; this prevented individual thresholds with very high uncertainties from dominating the overall results. A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a significant effect of eccentricity (
F(2,12) = 6.04,
p = .049). Standard
t-tests would have underestimated significance levels due to high standard deviations in the differences between conditions, so, to compare the effects of eccentricity on our weighted mean data, we used the weighted group means to estimate the differences between conditions and Z scores to compare whether these differences significantly differed from zero. For the monocular viewing condition, there was no significant difference between thresholds for targets at 0° and 7.5° of retinal eccentricity (Z = 0.58,
p = .56), which were both around 3°. These thresholds were significantly lower than the 6.5° mean monocular threshold measured at 15° of retinal eccentricity (0°/15°: Z = 5.04,
p < .001; 7.5°/15°: Z = 4.79,
p < .001). The binocular thresholds at 0° and 7.5° were close to 4° and were also not significantly different (Z = 1.12,
p = .26), but the 10.7° mean binocular threshold at 15° of retinal eccentricity was significantly higher than at the other two positions (0°/15°: Z = 2.84,
p < .01; 7.5°/15°: Z = 3.15,
p < .01). Particularly at fixation, these thresholds were much higher than what we predicted from the previously published data. One reason is that Fendick and Westheimer's (
1983) subjects had extensive practice, and another is that stimulus properties such as spatial frequency affect the rate at which disparity thresholds increase with eccentricity (Siderov & Harwerth,
1995).