Analysis indicated that, for each observer, the occluded right eye remained essentially stable while the left eye was moved passively (see
Figure 1). The average horizontal velocities of the right eye ranged from 0.34 ± 0.13 deg/s to 0.94 ± 0.26 deg/s for three of the observers. Because of mechanical interference between the two channels of the SRI Eyetracker, the interpupillary distance of the fourth observer was too small to obtain reliable eye recordings of both eyes simultaneously.
Previous studies indicated that the neural compensation for motion smear is asymmetrical, occurring preferentially for relative motion of the target in the opposite direction of eye movement (Tong et al.,
2006,
2007). As discussed in these papers, we interpret this asymmetrical compensation to reflect a preference by the visual system to perceive targets as clear only if the targets are interpreted as potentially stationary in the world. Here, we determined the extent of perceived motion smear for three conditions:
-
when the laser target moved with the direction of eye push,
-
when the laser target moved against the direction of eye push, and
-
during motion of the target with no eye push.
The results show a significant difference among these conditions (see
Figure 2A; repeated-measures ANOVA:
F [2,6] = 9.54,
p = 0.025). Specifically, the duration of perceived motion smear is approximately 40 ms less when the spot moves
against compared to
with the direction of passive eye movement (
F [1,6] = 15.32,
p = 0.015) or compared to when the eye remains stationary (
F [1,6] = 13.24,
p = 0.019). In contrast, when the laser spot moves
with the direction of passive eye movement, the extent of perceived motion smear is similar to that during no eye push (
F [1,6] = 0.076,
p = 0.73). The selective reduction of perceived smear during
against motion is not attributable to an effect of the laser spot on the observers' passive eye movements, as paired
t tests indicate no significant difference in passive eye velocity according to the direction of laser-spot motion (range of
p values across observers = 0.08–0.97). The scatter plots of the data of the four individual observers in
Figure 2B indicate that the extent of perceived smear decreases with increasing eye velocity when the target moves against the direction of eye movement, which is consistent with previous studies (Tong et al.,
2006,
2007).
In the second experiment, the same observers reported the extent of perceived motion smear during smooth pursuit and fixation. In agreement with previous studies (Tong et al.,
2006,
2007),
Figure 3A indicates that the extent of perceived motion smear is significantly smaller when the target moves
against the direction of an ongoing pursuit eye movement compared with when the target moves in the same direction as pursuit (
F[1,6] = 8.35,
p = 0.036) or during fixation (
F[1,6] = 10.62,
p = 0.024).
Figure 3B shows that the relationship between the eye speed during pursuit and the extent of perceived smear is qualitatively similar to that shown during passive eye movement in
Figure 2B. Although the slopes fit to the four observers' aggregate data differ by a factor of 2.5 in the first and second experiments (
Figures 2B and
3B), a linear regression analysis using a dummy variable showed no significant difference between the results (
t[df=161] = 1.09,
p = 0.28).
1 Further, the slopes of the lines fit to the two data sets are closer quantitatively (slope during passive eye movement = −6.39; slope during pursuit = −10.32;
t[df=97] = 0.39,
p = 0.70) if the eye velocities in the passive eye-movement condition are restricted to be in the same range as those sampled during pursuit.
The extent of perceived motion smear in the “no-eye-push” condition in the first experiment is similar to that in the “fixation” condition in the second experiment ( t [df=3] = 0.68, p = 0.54). The similarity of these results indicates that the characteristics of the matching stimuli used in the two experiments (two separated dots in Experiment 1 vs. a line in Experiment 2) have no significant impact on the results. We therefore compared the efficacy of extra-retinal signals in reducing perceived motion smear in each experiment by calculating the difference between the smear reported in the “no-eye-push” (or “fixation”) and the “against” eye-movement conditions and then dividing this difference by the extent of perceived motion smear during the “no-eye-push” or “fixation” condition. This analysis revealed that the efficacy of extra-retinal signals during smooth pursuit (mean = 22%) is not significantly different from the efficacy during passive eye movement (mean = 27%, paired t-test: t [df=3] = 0.46, p = 0.68).