The neural representation of light from a location in visual space takes into account the spatial and temporal context of other light nearby. A well-known example is chromatic induction, in which the color in one area is influenced by surrounding light (De Valois, Webster, De Valois, & Lingelbach,
1986; Monnier & Shevell,
2003). One instance of induction is color contrast, in which the appearance of an area shifts away from the color in the surround (Krauskopf, Zaidi, & Mandler,
1986; Smith, Jin, & Pokorny,
2001; Zaidi, Yoshimi, Flanigan, & Canova,
1992). For example, a central square appears redder when viewed within a green-appearing surround than within an achromatic surround. In variegated natural environments, context plays an important role in determining color appearance.
Visual perception of temporally varying light is well studied (e.g. Bowker,
1983; Kelly,
1975; Lee, Pokorny, Smith, Martin, & Valberg,
1990; Pokorny, Smith, & Lutze,
1989), but comparatively little research examines how the properties of temporally varying surrounding light affect perceived temporal variation induced within a steady uniform region (De Valois et al.,
1986; Kinney,
1965,
1967; Rossi & Paradiso,
1996). A consistent finding in earlier work is that the perceived modulation depth induced within a central region is severely attenuated at temporal inducing frequencies above about 3 Hz (D'Antona & Shevell,
2006; De Valois et al.,
1986; Rossi & Paradiso,
1996). This differs markedly from the perceived modulation depth of the temporally varying surround itself, which is first attenuated at a far higher temporal frequency (De Valois et al.,
1986). Thus, perceived induced modulation seems to rely on a slower neural mechanism than perceived modulation of a physically varying light.
Temporally varying neural signals before the site of induction may be important for understanding how temporally varying light contributes to perception. Though induced modulation is attenuated above ∼3 Hz, physiological and psychophysical studies clearly demonstrate temporally varying neural signals can follow to much higher frequencies (Lee et al.,
1990). Higher-frequency neural responses preceding the site of induction may have important consequences for induced temporal modulation, even though they are not directly perceived. For example, in brightness induction, higher temporal frequencies can contribute to perceived motion using a grating-induction stimulus (Blakeslee & McCourt,
2008).
The research here investigated whether neural mechanisms mediating induced temporal modulation were driven by a larger range of stimulus temporal frequencies than reported previously. Previous studies examine induced temporal modulation using one inducing temporal frequency at a time within a surround. These measured characteristics of induced temporal modulation would generalize to any temporal waveform by assuming a linear neural response. If the linearity assumption is invalid, however, then a temporal frequency that induces no temporal variation when presented alone may induce temporal variation when a component of a more complex waveform. Consider, for example, a stimulus that is the superposition of two sine waves at frequencies
f 1 and
f 2 (rightmost graph,
Figure 1A). It periodically reaches a peak at the difference frequency of its individual frequency components ∣
f 2 −
f 1∣. Superposition of this frequency pair is the physical summation of two sine waves, so power is only at the two individual frequency components (
f 1 and
f 2). If this stimulus drives a nonlinear neural response, however, then the output from the nonlinearity can contain power at temporal frequencies not present in the stimulus. Two new temporal frequencies in a nonlinear response are the difference frequency ∣
f 2 −
f 1∣ and the sum frequency (
f 1 +
f 2), which are shown in
Figure 1B. In the case of induced temporal modulation, each frequency
f 1 and
f 2 presented individually may be too high to induce temporal modulation; when presented simultaneously, however, and with a nonlinearity preceding the site of induction,
f 1 and
f 2 result in a new lower-temporal-frequency component at the difference frequency. This difference frequency may be low enough to induce temporal modulation, even though each component in the stimulus is at a frequency too high to cause induction. This hypothesis was tested in three experiments.