The results of Experiment 1 confirmed previous findings (Tadin et al.,
2003) obtained in a very similar discrimination paradigm. Performance in the direction discrimination task increased with increasing stimulus size for low-contrast stimuli and decreased with increasing stimulus size for high-contrast stimuli. Tadin et al. (
2003) interpreted their results as an effect of the inhibitory center-surround influences that are observed in about 50% of the neurons in area MT (e.g. Born,
2000). This interpretation was supported by the finding that the inhibitory surrounds of MT neurons are far weaker when low-contrast stimuli are used (Pack et al.,
2005).
Although our results confirmed the results of Tadin et al. in a similar paradigm, the relationship between stimulus size and discrimination performance changed dramatically when we manipulated the duration between the appearance of the stimulus and the motion step. Increasing this MOA beyond 35 ms decreased the size dependence of direction discrimination, and for MOA beyond ∼100 ms the size dependence completely disappeared. Tadin et al. (
2003) reported size effects using a MOA of 50 ms, and so their results are not inconsistent with ours. Thus we suggest that masking caused by the onset (and to a smaller degree by the offset) of the stimulus influences the subsequent percept of motion, and that this phenomenon contributes to the effects of size and contrast on motion perception.
At the same time we do not wish to imply that all effects of psychophysical surround suppression are due to abrupt stimulus onsets since similar psychophysical effects were obtained with long-duration stimuli by measuring motion after-effects (Tadin, Paffen, Blake, & Lappin,
2008), perception of counterphasing gratings (Aaen-Stockdale, Thompson, Huang, & Hess,
2008), and binocular rivalry (Paffen, Tadin, te Pas, Blake, & Verstraten,
2006), and there is little reason to believe that any of these measures would be affected by stimulus onsets. It is not clear to what extent these latter approaches engage the same mechanisms as the earlier experiments involving brief motion stimuli (Betts et al.,
2009; Tadin et al.,
2003), as neurophysiological data with comparable stimuli are not (to our knowledge) available. Moreover, we recently found that brief motion stimuli similar to those used in our experiments and those of Tadin et al. (
2003) preferentially activate MT neurons that have strong surround suppression (Churan et al.,
2008b). This is consistent with a role for inhibitory surrounds in the phenomenon of psychophysical surround suppression. Thus temporal factors such as abrupt onsets appear to interact with spatial factors such as stimulus size in a rather complex way, and we are currently exploring this issue with additional MT recordings (Churan et al.,
2008a).
Our results in Experiments 2 suggest that psychophysical surround suppression, as measured with brief stimuli, may share a common mechanism with forward masking. In studies on masking the presentation of one stimulus reduces sensitivity to another stimulus (Breitmeyer & Öğmen,
2006), even when the second stimulus differs in phase from the first (Georgeson,
1988). However, in most masking experiments increasing the duration of the mask stimulus does not restore sensitivity for the test stimulus (Georgeson,
1987), as it did in our experiment. Moreover, these experiments have generally used a fixed stimulus size and detection rather than discrimination as a measure of performance. Nevertheless our results on stimulus timing are generally consistent with the idea that forward (and to a lesser extent backward) masking is important to psychophysical surround suppression of motion stimuli.
In the space domain, there are divergent reports about the link between stimulus size and masking. In particular several studies have found that increasing the size of a mask can lead to a decrease of masking (Herzog & Koch,
2001; Wehrhahn, Li, & Westheimer,
1996; Yu & Levi,
2000) which is not consistent with a role for surround suppression in these masking processes. However, other studies have found that increasing the size of the mask increases the strength of masking effects (e.g. Bonneh & Sagi,
1999). These divergent results are likely related to the different stimuli used in the different experiments, which suggests that the spatial layout of the mask and target are more important than the overall size of the mask.
One interpretation that may provide a unified account of our results and previous work on masking involves mechanisms of short-term adaptation. Adaptation following the sudden onset of a stimulus has been observed psychophysically (e.g. Crawford,
1947; Pokorny, Sun, & Smith,
2003; Yeh, Lee, & Kremers,
1996), and it is thought to have effects similar to masking for a wide range of stimuli (Georgeson,
1987). In our experiments, short-term adaptation would be induced by the presence of the stationary grating during the MOA (Müller, Metha, Krauskopf, & Lennie,
1999). This would be particularly useful in the discrimination task if the sudden onset of the stimulus saturated the nonlinear responses of direction-selective mechanisms. In this case the difference between preferred and null-direction responses would be relatively small, and the MOA would permit the visual system to lower contrast gain, thereby becoming more sensitive to motion direction. This hypothesis requires (compressive) nonlinear processing of contrast, which is consistent with observations in the magnocellular processing stream (Maunsell, Nealey, & DePriest,
1990). However, it is not clear how it relates to the significant (though weaker) effects of the MTA.
Although our results may be interpreted in the context of contrast adaptation, any such adaptation effect must be specific to the orientation of the masking stimulus. In Experiment 4 we showed that improved performance for large stimuli depended strongly on the orientation of the stationary grating that preceded the motion step. Relative to performance with a short MOA, an orientation difference smaller than approximately 15 degrees improved discrimination performance, while a larger tilt worsened it. This effect is consistent with observations on cross-orientation contrast masking (Saarela & Herzog,
2008), although in our experiments the influence of the mask on motion perception was substantially stronger, with orthogonal masks rendering observers completely insensitive to motion direction.
In general we have interpreted our results under the assumption that observers were attempting to detect the motion of the stimulus. However, another possibility is that observers used a strategy that involved tracking the displacement of the grating, rather than measuring the motion
per se (Lee, Wehrhahn, Westheimer, & Kremers,
1993; Westheimer,
1978). Indeed recent work has shown that the perception of very brief motion stimuli is limited by spatial acuity, and that psychophysical surround suppression disappears at very low speeds (Lappin et al.,
2009). Visual mechanisms that track stimulus displacement are likely to differ from those that detect first-order motion (Seiffert & Cavanagh,
1998), and so it is possible that observers switched from a motion detection strategy at short MOA to a displacement detection strategy at MOA longer than ∼100 ms. Our results in
Figure 4 showing higher displacement thresholds for peripheral stimulation are consistent with this idea. This explanation is also compatible with a role for onset transients in psychophysical surround suppression, but it makes the additional prediction that brain regions involved in measuring displacement will exhibit weaker surround inhibition than those that detect motion.