Despite its difficulties, many nocturnal animals—including insects—rely on vision for the tasks of daily life (Warrant,
2007,
2008a). Insects have compound eyes, an eye type constructed from an assembly of repeated optical units, the “ommatidia” (
Figure 1A). Nocturnal insects, including most moths and many beetles, typically have refracting superposition compound eyes, a design that allows single photoreceptors in the retina to receive focussed light from hundreds (and in some extreme cases, thousands) of corneal facet lenses (
Figure 1C). This design represents a vast improvement in sensitivity over the apposition compound eye (
Figure 1B), a design in which single photoreceptors receive light only from the single corneal facet lens residing in the same ommatidium. Not surprisingly, apposition eyes are typical of diurnal insects active in bright sunlight, such as bees, flies, butterflies, and dragonflies. Interestingly, despite their disadvantages for vision in dim light, some nocturnal insects have apposition compound eyes. In tropical rainforests, for instance, the competition for resources and the threat of predation are fierce, and many species of bees and wasps—all with apposition eyes—have evolved a nocturnal lifestyle, an evolutionary transition that is probably relatively recent (Wcislo et al.,
2004). Animals with apposition eyes have evolved a number of adaptations that improve sensitivity in dim light (Barlow, Kaplan, Renninger, & Saito,
1987; Warrant,
2007,
2008b) and that allow visually guided behavior at lowest light intensities (Barlow & Powers,
2003; Warrant et al.,
2004). For example, the apposition eyes of the nocturnal sweat bee,
Megalopta genalis, possess larger corneal facet lenses and wider rhabdoms than those of closely related diurnal bees, and thus enjoy an increased optical sensitivity (Greiner, Ribi, & Warrant,
2004). Further improvements in visual sensitivity are likely to come from enhanced visual gain in the photoreceptors (Barlow, Bolanowski, & Brachman,
1977; Barlow et al.,
1987; Frederiksen, Wcislo, & Warrant,
2008) and from a strategy of visual summation in space and time (Theobald, Greiner, Wcislo, & Warrant,
2006; Warrant et al.,
2004). Morphological, electrophysiological, and theoretical evidences (Warrant,
2008b) suggest that widely arborizing second-order cells in the lamina (the LMC cells) might mediate a spatial summation of visual signals generated in the retina (Greiner, Ribi, & Warrant,
2005; Greiner, Ribi, Wcislo, & Warrant,
2004).