For now we are looking for models that produced fair predictions of both the slope of the psychometric function and the level of summation. However, as it is straightforward to reduce the level of summation in the models by reducing the extent of pooling (Meese & Summers,
2007) we are not concerned, at this stage, if models overpredicted summation. The small filled circles (red and black) in
Figure 3a show the 62 model predictions (some of which superimpose). From visual inspection, the models clearly fall into two groups. Those where the slopes of the psychometric functions are far too shallow (the steepest slope is
β = 1.5) and those where the slopes of the psychometric functions are quite steep (the shallowest slope is
β = 2.3, though
β ≈ 3 is more typical). Within this second group, summation is too low in most cases. In fact, there are only four models for which summation is greater than the lower confidence limit of the observer with the weakest level of summation (SAW). These are shown by the slightly larger (red) points in
Figure 3a, labeled A, B, C, and D (points C and D superimpose). In each of these models, the slopes of the psychometric functions are quite steep (e.g.,
β > 3). Only this group of four models
1 produced predictions consistent with the data according to the rejection criteria outlined above. The results in
Figure 3a reject the other 58 models. In fact, from
Figure 3a, model B is arguably marginal. However, it is the most successful model that involves the MAX operation over area, which is akin to spatial probability summation when it follows noise, as it does here (Tyler & Chen,
2000). And as models of spatial probability summation have a long history (e.g., Robson & Graham,
1981), we retain this model arrangement in our shortlist and further analyses below.