Humans are impressively sensitive to biological motion, even when the depiction of the agent is reduced to a moving constellation of dots (Johansson,
1973). Such stimuli, commonly referred to as point light walkers (PLWs), are not only robust to noise (Blake,
1993; Cutting, Moore, & Morrison,
1988; Neri, Morrone, & Burr,
1998), but carry information about gender and identity (Cutting & Kozlowski,
1977; Mather & Murdoch,
1994; Pollick, Kay, Heim, & Stringer,
2005). These stimuli usually depict a person walking on the spot and, despite a typical gait cycle lasting 1–2 seconds, the duration required for detection is most commonly cited as around 200 ms (Johansson,
1976). Ideas about what mediates this rapid detection can be split into those which emphasize the role of motion (Casile & Giese,
2005; Thurman & Grossman,
2008), and those which highlight the influence of the form inherent in the configuration of the dots (Beintema & Lappe,
2002; Bertenthal & Pinto,
1994; Hiris,
2007).
Form and motion are inherently linked in the PLW stimulus, and recent work has focused on the interaction between these two sources of information (Garcia & Grossman,
2008; Hiris,
2007). It was initially suggested that the PLW lacked any form signals and contained only the motion information of the joints (Johansson,
1973). The aptitude demonstrated in biological motion tasks lead to suggestions of specialized mechanisms existing to process biological motion on a perceptual (Troje & Westhoff,
2006) or neural (Giese & Poggio,
2003) level. However Hiris (
2007) demonstrated that, when form information is added to non-biological stimuli, performance improves in line with that of the PLW. The PLW was conceived as a stimulus free of “interference from figural perception” (Johansson,
1973, p. 201) but this misrepresents the nature of the information within the PLW. Static form has certainly been degraded by reducing the stimulus to a sparse array of dots, but the coherence in the relationships between these dots make the PLW qualitatively different from displays of nonbiological motion lacking such relationships. Hiris (
2007) suggested that the lack of such relationships in comparable nonbiological motion may be the reason why perceptual systems appear attuned to biological motion stimuli.
Motion information in point light displays can be characterized as existing at two levels. Each point of light has its own local motion signal but integration of these signals produces relative motion, which constitutes an independent source of information (Casile & Giese,
2005). Although this information is dynamically presented, it has been argued that its importance is due to it revealing the articulation of the stimulus (Mather, Radford, & West,
1992) and should be considered “motion-mediated structure” (Troje,
2002, p. 372) rather than motion information. While purely dynamic information, such as the local motions of the individual dots, can be presented outside the context of the bodily action, the correct integration of these signals relies on intact spatiotemporal relationships between the dots. This integrative motion signal forms the basis of the “opponent motion” commonly referred to in the biological motion literature (Casile & Giese,
2005; Thurman & Grossman,
2008). It can, however, be more widely seen to underlie the grouping or recognition of any two or more dots through some knowledge of their relative motion characteristics. The effect of the integrative motion signal is therefore to imply both the structure and the hierarchy of connections within the global form of the PLW.
The potency of a signal based on the relative motions of two or more dots may be subject to changes in that relationship (Casile & Giese,
2005). Such changes, along with changes in the form information presented by a PLW, occur in the course of the gait cycle (Mather & Murdoch,
1994; Thurman & Grossman,
2008). The impact of these variations can be studied by comparing detection performance throughout the gait cycle. Thurman & Grossman used short presentations of a PLW embedded in a noise mask created from the constituent dots of the stimulus to measure these changes. Such an approach provides insight into the ability to segregate the stimulus from a noise mask that shares the same characteristics at a local level, but may be limited by potential problems with noise mask tasks: Beintema and Lappe (
2002) have previously suggested that processing of the stimulus is not all that is being measured in noise-based biological motion tasks, as segregation from the background could plausibly permit successful detection without an understanding of the stimulus.
Thurman and Grossman's (
2008) results show that the PLW is most detectable in noise when the limbs are crossing the midline of the body. During this phase, the profile of the PLW is a roughly vertical band of dots with little discernable bodily form. At the same point in the gait cycle, Thurman & Grossman argue that the integrative motion signal is at its greatest due to the relative motion signal produced by opposing limbs crossing. This inverse relationship between the strength of form and motion signals led them to interpret their results as evidence for the reliance of biological motion perception on motion information.
On the other hand the importance of form information is emphasized by Hiris (
2007) who, also using a noise threshold design, found that noise tolerance for non-biological motion with form was similar to that for biological motion. Both this study and Thurman and Grossman's used the trajectories of the stimulus' constituent dots on a trial-by-trial basis to create the noise mask. The efficiency of a noise mask is dictated by the similarity of the stimulus to the noise masking it and, as previous studies have shown, noise generated by scrambling the PLW is the most effective noise for biological motion tasks (Cutting et al.,
1988; Thompson, Hansen, Hess, & Troje,
2007). However, such noise does not contain the spatiotemporal relationships between the dots characteristic to biological motion stimuli and so cannot mask this source of information. Arguably this is evidence for the influence of global processing in such a task, as in this case the noise matches the local information of the stimulus (Thompson et al.,
2007). The absence of the motion relationships among the noise dots is possibly more relevant to the present study: variability in these relationships over the gait cycle will be reflected in the effectiveness of the mask over that cycle.
The nature of noise mask tasks and the specific noise used to mask the stimulus may influence the discriminations being measured and could help explain the contradictory ideas that either form or motion information underpins biological motion processing. Careful experimental design and stimulus creation is essential because artifacts in the data, caused by task effects, can dramatically affect the results and their interpretation. The challenge of designing an elegant task with which to gather a clean measure of a specific perceptual ability becomes more difficult as the stimulus needed to elicit the ability becomes more complex. Attempts to separate form and motion information in the PLW are hampered by the intimate relationship between the two in biological motion stimuli (Troje, Westhoff, & Lavrov,
2005). Therefore not only is it important to consider the informational content of both the signal and noise, but also to compare performance across a number of tasks probing the same question. Such an approach will demonstrate if a particular measure or effect is task dependent.
An alternative to noise mask based detection tasks is a discrimination task using a control that has been scrambled in such a manner as to reduce recognition performance. By offsetting the path of each dot of the PLW by a random number of frames, the PLW is phase-scrambled. This control corrupts the temporal relationships between dots but preserves the local motion of the dots. Because the local motion trajectories of the stimulus and control are the same any discrimination must arise from the relationship between the component motions (i.e. motion integration). Such a control is widely employed within the literature (Ahlström, Blake, & Ahlström,
1997; Bertenthal & Pinto,
1994) and prevents the organization of the constituent dots into a walking person by destroying the internal coherence and phase relations of the dots, while maintaining the temporal and spatial characteristics of each individual dot at a local level. This task also allows for much shorter display durations than a noise mask task, allowing the variations in the gait cycle to be studied in greater detail. The contributions of static form and motion integration to biological motion processing can also be assessed by comparing performance against static presentations of the same discrimination task.
The reported temporal detection limit of 200 ms for a PLW (Johansson,
1976) is considerably shorter than the duration of a full gait cycle. Therefore, to prevent ceiling effects in PLW tasks, presentation times are regularly limited to a fraction of the total cycle. Common practice dictates that the portion of gait cycle displayed in any experimental interval is randomized throughout the experimental task. However recent research suggests that the form inherent in biological motion displays is a crucial source of information for human performance (Hiris,
2007; Lange, Georg, & Lappe,
2006). The periodic changes in form throughout the gait cycle present an opportunity to test the assumption that the strength of form information remains constant throughout the gait cycle. The following experiments were designed to address these issues. Our experiments examined the detection and discrimination of biological motion in briefly presented segments of gait cycle.
Our results may indicate that the perception of biological motion may be driven by form information, and that static form is supported by form expressed through integrative motion signals. However, we find that the choice of task affects this relationship, and therefore conclusions should be drawn cautiously. Different experimental designs and different actions could potentially lead to different interpretations of the relationship between form and motion information in the PLW. Biological motion, as represented by the PLW, is a complex visual stimulus and the types of information within are highly interrelated. Methodological details can dramatically influence the findings; and our results demonstrate the importance of using a number of approaches, and the convergent evidence they produce, to study biological motion.