With this extension the model can account for the accumulation of slow changes over the course of multiple minutes, and it can explain how this accumulation can terminate a prolonged period of stable perception. This is illustrated in
Figure A2, panels A and B. The two black dots in panel A represent two consecutive stimulus onsets, marked ‘1’ and ‘2.’ Percept
i gains dominance at onset 1, and the resulting accumulation of Σ
A i is reflected by the gray arrow marked ‘
i dominant’. After the stimulus disappears (white dot) Σ
A i decays as indicated by the arrow marked ‘blank’. These arrows reflect motions that are primarily due to the faster components of Σ
A i, which respond relatively quickly to perceptual dominance and stimulus removal. However, some accumulation of the slower components also takes place. This slower accumulation ensures that at the moment of the subsequent stimulus onset (marked ‘2’) the system state has shifted to the right with respect to the previous onset (marked ‘1’). Panel B shows how this sequence is repeated over many presentations, until slow components have lifted Σ
A i so far along the
x-axis that, at a stimulus appearance marked ‘
m,’ the system reaches the gray region. As a result percept
j gains dominance, which constitutes a perceptual alternation. In other words, although an elevated level of Σ
A i is initially responsible for the occurrence of stabilization, further accumulation of Σ
A i over the course of a stabilization period causes perception to alternate. This means that perceptual stabilization is a process that eventually terminates itself. Below we will detail why the alternations produced by this model occur at regular intervals, as observed experimentally.