May 2008
Volume 8, Issue 7
Free
Research Article  |   May 2008
Perceptual grouping and inverse fMRI activity patterns in human visual cortex
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision May 2008, Vol.8, 2. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/8.7.2
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Fang Fang, Daniel Kersten, Scott O. Murray; Perceptual grouping and inverse fMRI activity patterns in human visual cortex. Journal of Vision 2008;8(7):2. https://doi.org/10.1167/8.7.2.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure activity in human visual cortex, including a higher object processing area, the lateral occipital complex (LOC), and primary visual cortex (V1), in response to a perceptually bistable stimulus whose elements were perceived as either grouped into a shape or randomly arranged. We found activity increases in the LOC and simultaneous reductions of activity in V1 when the elements were perceived as a coherent shape. Consistent with a number of inferential models of visual processing, our results suggest that feedback from higher visual areas to lower visual areas serves to reduce activity during perceptual grouping. The implications of these findings with respect to these models are discussed.

Introduction
A defining characteristic of human visual perception is the ability to assemble complex visual features—sometimes spatially separated and partially occluded—into coherent, unified representations of objects and surfaces. Grouping processes can vastly simplify the description of a visual scene because multiple features can be assigned to a single “cause.” For example, multiple lines of the same orientation can be described as a single texture without needing to specify each element within the pattern. 
What are the neural mechanisms that underlie perceptual grouping? We have suggested (Murray, Schrater, & Kersten, 2004) that perceptual grouping involves increases in activity in higher visual areas that code for spatial patterns (e.g., objects, surfaces, and textures) along with decreases in activity in lower visual areas that code for local, individual elements of the pattern (e.g., local orientation or direction of motion). We propose that this inverse relationship in neural activity between higher and lower visual areas reflects an “efficient code” of visual information. As higher visual areas converge on a single, global hypothesis for the individual elements in a visual scene, lower visual areas no longer need to represent the individual elements. 
A variety of computational models propose mechanisms that include interactions between high-level and low-level representations of image feature that result in inverse activity patterns. Predictive coding models (Mumford, 1992; Rao & Ballard, 1999), for example, are one class of models that suggest that feedback may operate to reduce activity in lower areas. Predictive coding models usually posit a subtractive comparison between hypotheses generated in higher areas and incoming sensory input in lower areas. In these models, reduced activity occurs when the predictions of higher-level areas match incoming sensory information. Recent fMRI and MEG studies have provided evidence supporting these models (Furl, van Rijsbergen, Treves, Friston, & Dolan, 2007; Harrison, Stephan, Rees, & Friston, 2007; Summerfield et al., 2006, but see also Dumoulin & Hess, 2006). 
Consistent with feedback models, in an initial fMRI study, we observed signal increases in the lateral occipital complex (LOC)—a higher visual area well known for its involvement in shape and object processing (Grill-Spector, Kourtzi, & Kanwisher, 2001)—and concurrent signal reductions in primary visual cortex (V1) when visual elements were assembled into coherent shapes (Murray, Kersten, Olshausen, Schrater, & Woods, 2002). This inverse activity pattern between the LOC and V1 was observed both for images of lines and for images of moving dots that were arranged to form shapes (“grouped”) compared to when they were randomly assembled (“ungrouped”). Although we had performed a number of control studies showing that the results were not due to various stimulus differences between the grouped and the ungrouped stimulus conditions, completely ruling out such differences is difficult. 
One class of stimuli that control for image differences are image patterns with bistable perceptual properties. These stimuli provide a powerful tool for investigating the neural mechanisms underlying perceptual grouping because they have constant image features that spontaneously switch between being perceived as grouped into a single object and being perceived as ungrouped visual features. In the current experiment, we measured fMRI activity as subjects viewed a perceptually bistable stimulus with four moving line segments that were either perceived as a rigid diamond translating horizontally or perceived as ungrouped line segments. Earlier measurements (Murray et al., 2002) had shown that activity in V1 decreases when the line segments are perceptually grouped into a rigid diamond. The current study represents a significant advance in methodology, together with additional analyses of extrastriate areas. Across all of our subjects, we observed significant inverse activity patterns in the LOC and V1: Activity increases in the LOC during perceptual grouping were accompanied by activity decreases in V1. 
Methods
Subjects
A total of four healthy subjects (3 male and 1 female) participated in the experiments, all of whom had extensive experience as subjects in psychophysical and fMRI experiments. They were right-handed, reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and had no known neurological or visual disorders. Ages ranged from 26 to 32. They gave written, informed consent in accordance with the procedures and the protocols approved by the human subjects review committee of the University of Minnesota. 
Stimuli and designs
The main experiment was performed using a constant image sequence that formed a changing bistable percept with either grouped or ungrouped line segments. The stimulus was a line drawing of a diamond whose four corners were occluded by three vertical bars of the same color as the background (Figure 1A). The diamond moved at a constant horizontal speed of 1.3 deg/s and reversed direction every 1 s. A similar stimulus was used by Lorenceau and Shiffrar (1992). The line segments were 2.6° in length, and their centers were at 2.8°–3.9° eccentricity. The stimulus could be perceived either as a rigid diamond moving horizontally behind occluders (diamond, Figure 1B) or as individual line segments moving vertically (non-diamond, Figure 1C). The two percepts alternated and subjects indicated their perceptual state with a button press. A total of 5–6 400 s scans were performed for each subject. 
Figure 1
 
The stimulus was a horizontally translating diamond whose four corners were occluded by three vertical bars of the same color as the background (A). The four remaining line segments could be perceived as a rigid diamond moving horizontally (B) or as individual line segments moving vertically (C).
Figure 1
 
The stimulus was a horizontally translating diamond whose four corners were occluded by three vertical bars of the same color as the background (A). The four remaining line segments could be perceived as a rigid diamond moving horizontally (B) or as individual line segments moving vertically (C).
Retinotopic visual areas were defined by a standard method developed by Engel, Glover, and Wandell (1997) and Sereno et al. (1995). Two block-design scans were used to define the regions of interest (ROI). In one scan, a 10-Hz counterphase-flickering stimulus (Figure 2) was passively viewed to define the subregions of V1, V2, and V3 corresponding to the areas covered by the moving line segments. The scan consisted of ten 20-s stimulus blocks and ten 20-s blank intervals, which interleaved with each other. In the other scan, to localize object processing areas, subjects passively viewed images of intact and scrambled objects, which subtended 9.4° × 9.4° and were centered at the fixation. Images appeared at a rate of 2 Hz in blocks of 20 s. Intact and scrambled object blocks were repeated 10 times and interleaved with each other. 
Figure 2
 
A flickering checkered stimulus used for defining the subregion of V1 corresponding to the areas covered by the moving line segments.
Figure 2
 
A flickering checkered stimulus used for defining the subregion of V1 corresponding to the areas covered by the moving line segments.
MRI data acquisition
In the scanner, the stimuli were back-projected via a video projector (60 Hz) onto a translucent screen placed inside the scanner bore. Subjects viewed the stimuli through a mirror located above their eyes. The viewing distance was 92 cm. MRI data were collected using a 3-T Siemens Trio scanner with an eight-channel phase-array coil. BOLD signals were measured with an EPI (echo-planar imaging) sequence (TE: 30 ms; TR: 1000 ms; FOV: 22 × 22 cm2; matrix: 64 × 64; flip angle: 60; slice thickness: 5 mm; gap: 0 mm; number of slices: 10; slice orientation: axial). The bottom slice was positioned at the bottom of the temporal lobes. A high-resolution 3D structural data set (3D MPRAGE; 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 resolution) was collected in the same session before the functional scans. All four subjects participated in two fMRI sessions for the retinotopic mapping experiment and the main experiment, respectively. 
MRI data processing and analysis
The anatomical volume for each subject in the retinotopic mapping session was transformed into the AC–PC space. The cortical surface was extracted and then inflated using BrainVoyager 2000. Functional volumes in all the sessions for each subject were preprocessed, which included 3D motion correction using SPM99, linear trend removal, and high-pass (0.015 Hz) (Smith et al., 1999) filtering using BrainVoyager 2000. The images were then aligned to the anatomical volume in the retinotopic mapping session and transformed into the AC–PC space. The first 10 s of BOLD signals were discarded to minimize transient magnetic-saturation effects. 
A GLM (general linear model) procedure was used for ROI analysis. The ROIs in V1, V2, and V3 were defined as areas that responded more strongly to the flickering checkered stimulus (Figure 2) than blank interval (p < 0.01, corrected) and confined by the retinotopic boundaries defined by the retinotopic mapping experiment. LOC was defined as areas in the occipital cortex that respond more strongly to object images than scrambled object images (p < 0.01, corrected). With this contrast, some temporal object areas (TOA) were also found to be sensitive to object images (Fang & He, 2005; Grill-Spector, 2003), which have been suggested to be at a higher position than the LOC in the hierarchy of visual system (Grill-Spector & Malach, 2001). TOA was defined as areas of activation in response to objects that were in more anterior and ventral locations than the LOC. 
The event-related BOLD signals were calculated separately for each subject and each ROI. For each event-related scan, the time course of MR signal intensity was extracted by averaging the data from all the voxels within the pre-defined ROI, transformed into percent signal changes, and event-related averaged according to subject's response. Finally, the event-related signals were averaged across scans. 
Eye movement recording
Eye movements were recorded at 60 Hz with an iView X RED eye tracker (SensoMotoric Instruments GmbH, Teltow, Germany) in a psychophysics lab for two subjects when they viewed the same stimuli as those in the magnet. 
Results
We measured cortical activities in both earlier and higher visual areas while subjects viewed a bistable stimulus that was either perceived as a moving diamond behind occluders or as individual moving line segments. Importantly, only the percept and not the stimulus features changed during this experiment. The perceptual states switched every several seconds. The mean durations of diamond and non-diamond percepts were 6.3 s and 7.3 s, respectively. Figure 3 shows the frequency histograms of durations for the diamond and the non-diamond percepts. We find that the data are well fit with a Gamma distribution, which has been well documented in another bistable visual phenomenon—binocular rivalry (Kovács, Papathomas, Yang, & Fehér, 1996). This suggests that these two types of bistable perceptions have similar temporal dynamics. 
Figure 3
 
Histograms of durations for the diamond (left) and non-diamond (right) percepts. Data are fitted using a Gamma function (smooth black lines).
Figure 3
 
Histograms of durations for the diamond (left) and non-diamond (right) percepts. Data are fitted using a Gamma function (smooth black lines).
Activities in V1 and LOC showed significant changes during perceptual switches but in inverse patterns (Figure 4). Activity in V1 was significantly reduced when a coherently moving diamond was perceived and significantly increased when visual elements were not perceived as part of a shape, which is consistent with previous observations (Murray et al., 2002). On the other hand, activity in the LOC was significantly increased when subjects perceived a diamond and significantly decreased when they perceived four individual line segments. Paired t tests showed that the difference between the peak and the trough during perceptual switches reached a significant level in both V1 (t = 10.8, p < 0.01) and LOC (t = 9.5, p < 0.01). Compared with the fMRI signal in V1 and LOC, other visual areas showed a weaker or little modulation by the bistable percept (Figure 5). Specifically, V2 and the TOA showed a similar pattern as V1 and the LOC respectively, although with a smaller amplitude. V3 did not show a significant modulation associated with perceptual grouping. 
Figure 4
 
The average fMRI signals in the LOC and V1 during perceptual switches from each of the four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across scans.
Figure 4
 
The average fMRI signals in the LOC and V1 during perceptual switches from each of the four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across scans.
Figure 5
 
The average fMRI signals in V1, V2, V3, LOC, and TOA during perceptual switches averaged across four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across subjects.
Figure 5
 
The average fMRI signals in V1, V2, V3, LOC, and TOA during perceptual switches averaged across four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across subjects.
Eye movements are a possible confound. We monitored two subjects' eye positions. Figure 6 shows the frequency histograms of horizontal and vertical eye positions during the diamond and non-diamond percepts. The data show that their eye movements were small and there was no significant difference in the distribution of eye position between the diamond percept and the non-diamond percept. Also, further statistical analyses confirmed that both horizontal and vertical mean eye positions did not significantly deviate from the fixation point during both the diamond percept and the non-diamond percept. These results suggest that it is unlikely that our results could be significantly confounded by eye movements. 
Figure 6
 
Histograms of horizontal and vertical eye positions after removing blinks and artifacts during the diamond and non-diamond percepts from two subjects (the upper row for one subject and the lower row for the other).
Figure 6
 
Histograms of horizontal and vertical eye positions after removing blinks and artifacts during the diamond and non-diamond percepts from two subjects (the upper row for one subject and the lower row for the other).
Discussion
Our results demonstrate a significant and reliable pattern of fMRI activity associated with perceptual grouping: when moving line segments were perceived as a single, translating object, activity increased in the LOC and decreased in V1 compared to when the same line segments were perceived as ungrouped. The LOC activity pattern is expected because this cortical region is known to be shape-selective (e.g., Kourtzi & Kanwisher, 2001). The V1 activity pattern is consistent with our earlier finding (Murray et al., 2002). Taken together, these results suggest that feedback from higher visual areas serves to reduce activity in earlier visual areas during perceptual grouping. 
Although our earlier study (Murray et al., 2002) included a condition using a similar bistable “translating diamond,” the current study represents a significant advance in methodology and analysis. Here we used an independently defined, retinotopically specific localizer for V1. Thus, we are confident that the modulations in the fMRI signal that we observed occurred in the retinotopic representation of the stimulus and not in immediately adjacent retinotopic regions (e.g., artifacts due to “blood-flow steal”). In addition, due to limited slice-selection, our previous report using the translating diamond only made measurements in V1. Here, using an independent localizer for the LOC, we show significant changes that inversely reflect the pattern of activity observed in V1. Finally, the current study employs event-related averaging that characterizes the temporal dynamics and the magnitudes of V1 and LOC changes in more detail than our previous report. 
In addition to the LOC and V1, we analyzed the fMRI signal in V2, V3, and TOA, none of which showed the kinds of signal changes observed in the LOC and V1. The small modulation observed in TOA likely has a straightforward explanation—this region is considered to be at a relatively high level in the visual hierarchy and simple geometric shapes (e.g., the diamond) are unlikely to evoke much activity in this region. While V2 had a similar pattern of activity as V1, its amplitude was significantly reduced. V3 essentially had no change in signal in response to perceptual transitions. These observations are important as they point to a potentially unique computational role for V1 in perceptual grouping. 
Given the convincing empirical demonstration of inverse activity patterns in V1 and the LOC, the current findings raise important theoretical questions centered on the interpretation of the decreases in the fMRI signal in V1 when the line segments were perceptually grouped. First, what implications does the measurement technique have on the interpretation? As is well known, changes in the fMRI signal represent multiple hemodynamic processes related to multiple underlying physiological causes (Logothetis & Wandell, 2004). Although strong correlations between the fMRI response and the neural spike rate have been reported (e.g., Logothetis, Pauls, Augath, Trinath, & Oeltermann, 2001; Rees, Friston, & Koch, 2000), our observation of a reduced fMRI signal in V1 may be a manifestation of subthreshold and/or inhibitory processes in addition to a reduction in spiking activity. Other, more direct, techniques are required to resolve this question. 
Second, do the anti-correlations between the LOC and V1 necessarily mean a direct interaction between the two areas? More specifically, are the reductions observed in V1 necessarily caused by feedback from the LOC? Although correlations do not necessarily imply a causal relationship, if the LOC is maintaining a representation of the grouped elements and if the changes in V1 are due to changes in perception, it would imply at least an indirect relationship between the two regions. However, the only conclusive way to answer this question is to selectively remove feedback connections to V1. 
Third, and the perhaps most difficult question, are the reductions in V1 necessary for the perception of the diamond? Although a strong argument could be made that the modulations observed in the LOC—a region well known for shape perception—underlie the change in perception, making a similar argument for V1 is more difficult. V1 has traditionally been thought to maintain a veridical representation of retinal information. Consequently, a stimulus that has physically constant features—as with the translating diamond—is not generally expected to change V1 activity. We consider several alternative accounts of the potential functional significance of the V1 signal changes. 
On one end of the spectrum of possibilities, the changes in V1 might not be functionally significant. For example, fMRI measurements of V1 have shown reliable signal changes associated with spatial attention. Is it possible that the changes we observed simply reflect incidental shifts in spatial attention that occur during perceptual transitions? This explanation would require that subjects directed their spatial attention away from the line segments when they perceived the diamond, relative to the non-diamond condition. There is no reason to believe that these shifts occurred. In fact, our subjects claimed that they needed to focus their attention on the line segments in order to perceive the diamond. However, future studies that explicitly manipulate spatial attention and its effect on perceptual grouping and the fMRI signal are warranted. 
Along similar lines, the argument could be made that the differences in V1 and LOC activity might simply reflect attention to the features (“diamond” vs. “ungrouped line segments”) that result from the different perceptual states. For example, when subjects perceived ungrouped line segments they might have attended to this feature of the stimulus, consequently leading to more activity in V1 because it is presumably specialized for processing this feature. In contrast, when subjects perceived the diamond they might have attended to its overall shape leading to more activity in the LOC because of its specialization in shape processing. On one hand, attention to features is part of the process. During the perception of the diamond, subjects are certainly “attending to the diamond-ness” and separating the role of attention—which is directly tied to perceptual awareness—would be very difficult in our experimental setup. However, there is empirical evidence which renders a simple feature-based attention explanation unlikely. First, we observed notably diminished (V2) and abolished (V3) modulation of the fMRI signal in other early visual areas. There is no a priori reason to believe that these areas are any less specialized for the features of the “non-diamond” than V1. Second, Buracas, Fine, and Boynton (2005) compared fMRI responses in early visual cortex as subjects switched attention between different features (contrast vs. speed) of a moving grating. They found no modulation of the fMRI signal in any early visual area (V1, V2, V3, and MT) as a function of feature-based attention when, in theory, it might be expected. For example, early visual cortex is highly sensitive to contrast but attending to that feature did not modulate the fMRI signal. However, given the differences in underlying features in the Buracas et al. study (contrast and speed) compared to our study (grouping of line segments) to fully address the potential contribution of feature-based attention will require future direct empirical tests. Such an experiment might alternate attention between local versus global elements of simple shapes (such as the diamond) and measure activity in both lower and higher visual areas. 
An alternative interpretation of the decrease in V1 activity is that it might not have a direct functional significance but reveal a general metabolic efficiency constraint placed on neural processing. Spiking activity is metabolically expensive (Lennie, 2003) and there may be a general strategy to minimize neural activity whenever possible. For example, if one cortical area can represent the visual stimulus, another area should not. In our case, when the line segments form a representation that can be maintained in the LOC, V1 may participate less in the representation simply to minimize overall activity. Although sparseness constraints have been shown to have important theoretical implications related to the emergence of receptive field properties within a cortical area (Olshausen & Field, 1996), the implications of extending this principle to between areas are less clear. 
Finally, the reductions in V1 activity observed during perceptual grouping may reveal important functional mechanisms of visual information processing. One such mechanism, mentioned in the Introduction section, is predictive coding (Mumford, 1992; Rao & Ballard, 1999). Predictive coding models posit that higher areas are actively attempting to “explain” activity patterns in lower areas via feedback projections. Because most predictive coding models include a subtractive comparison between the hypotheses formed in higher areas and the incoming sensory input represented in lower areas, the overall effect of feedback may be to reduce activity in lower areas. Specifically, reduced activity in lower visual areas would occur whenever the predictions of higher-level areas match incoming sensory information. In the case of the translating diamond, when the LOC maintains a representation of a grouped shape, this “expectation” or “understanding” of the image features is sent back to V1 and removed, resulting in less activity. When the LOC is unable to form such an understanding (i.e., when they are perceived as ungrouped), these feedback processes are not occurring and there is consequently more activity in V1. 
In summary, although our results are consistent with a number of theoretical interpretations, they demonstrate that perceptual grouping involves activity modulations at multiple stages of the visual hierarchy. The two areas considered in detail here—the LOC and the V1—correspond to areas that are known to represent global shape and local visual features, respectively. Importantly, the activity patterns in these areas are inversely related and suggest that perceptual grouping involves both increases and decreases in activity in the human visual system. 
Acknowledgments
This work is supported by NIH grant R01 EY015261-01. The 3-T scanner at the University of Minnesota, Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, was supported by NCRR P41 008079 and P30 NS057091 and by the MIND Institute. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Scott O. Murray. 
Email: somurray@u.washington.edu. 
Address: Department of Psychology, Box 351525, University of Washington, Seattle WA 98195, USA. 
References
Buracas, G. T. Fine, I. Boynton, G. M. (2005). The relationship between task performance and functional magnetic resonance imaging response. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 3023–3031. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dumoulin, S. O. Hess, R. F. (2006). Modulation of V1 activity by shape: Image-statistics or shape based perception?. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3654–3664. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Engel, S. A. Glover, G. H. Wandellv, B. A. (1997). Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 181–192. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fang, F. He, S. (2005). Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1380–1385. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Furl, N. Rijsbergen, N. J. Treves, A. Friston, K. J. Dolan, R. J. (2007). Experience-dependent coding of facial expression in superior temporal sulcus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 13485–13489. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grill-Spector, K. (2003). The neural basis of object perception. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 159–166. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grill-Spector, K. Kourtzi, Z. Kanwisher, N. (2001). The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Research, 41, 1409–1422. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grill-Spector, K. Malach, R. (2001). fMR-adaptation: A tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychologica, 107, 293–321. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Harrison, L. M. Stephan, K. E. Rees, G. Friston, K. J. (2007). Extra-classical receptive field effects measured in striate cortex with fMRI. Neuroimage, 34, 1199–1208. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kourtzi, Z. Kanwisher, N. (2001). Representation of perceived object shape by the human lateral occipital complex. Science, 293, 1506–1509. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kovács, I. Papathomas, T. V. Yang, M. Fehér, A. (1996). When the brain changes its mind: Interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 15508–15511. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lennie, P. (2003). The cost of cortical computation. Current Biology, 13, 493–497. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Logothetis, N. K. Pauls, J. Augath, M. Trinath, T. Oeltermann, A. (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature, 412, 150–157. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Logothetis, N. K. Wandell, B. A. (2004). Interpreting the BOLD signal. Annual Review of Physiology, 66, 735–769. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lorenceau, J. Shiffrar, M. (1992). The influence of terminators on motion integration across space. Vision Research, 32, 263–273. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mumford, D. (1992). On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biological Cybernetics, 66, 241–251. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murray, S. O. Kersten, D. Olshausen, B. A. Schrater, P. Woods, D. L. (2002). Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 15164–15169. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murray, S. O. Schrater, P. Kersten, D. (2004). Perceptual grouping and the interactions between visual cortical areas. Neural Networks, 17, 695–705. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Olshausen, B. A. Field, D. J. (1996). Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images. Nature, 381, 607–609. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rao, R. P. Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 79–87. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rees, G. Friston, K. Koch, C. (2000). A direct quantitative relationship between the functional properties of human and macaque V5. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 716–723. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sereno, M. I. Dale, A. M. Reppas, J. B. Kwong, K. K. Belliveau, J. W. Brady, T. J. (1995). Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science, 268, 889–893. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smith, A. M. Lewis, B. K. Ruttimann, U. E. Ye, F. Q. Sinnwell, T. M. Yang, Y. (1999). Investigation of low frequency drift in fMRI signal. Neuroimage, 9, 526–533. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Summerfield, C. Egner, T. Greene, M. Koechlin, E. Mangels, J. Hirsch, J. (2006). Predictive codes for forthcoming perception in the frontal cortex. Science, 314, 1311–1314. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1
 
The stimulus was a horizontally translating diamond whose four corners were occluded by three vertical bars of the same color as the background (A). The four remaining line segments could be perceived as a rigid diamond moving horizontally (B) or as individual line segments moving vertically (C).
Figure 1
 
The stimulus was a horizontally translating diamond whose four corners were occluded by three vertical bars of the same color as the background (A). The four remaining line segments could be perceived as a rigid diamond moving horizontally (B) or as individual line segments moving vertically (C).
Figure 2
 
A flickering checkered stimulus used for defining the subregion of V1 corresponding to the areas covered by the moving line segments.
Figure 2
 
A flickering checkered stimulus used for defining the subregion of V1 corresponding to the areas covered by the moving line segments.
Figure 3
 
Histograms of durations for the diamond (left) and non-diamond (right) percepts. Data are fitted using a Gamma function (smooth black lines).
Figure 3
 
Histograms of durations for the diamond (left) and non-diamond (right) percepts. Data are fitted using a Gamma function (smooth black lines).
Figure 4
 
The average fMRI signals in the LOC and V1 during perceptual switches from each of the four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across scans.
Figure 4
 
The average fMRI signals in the LOC and V1 during perceptual switches from each of the four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across scans.
Figure 5
 
The average fMRI signals in V1, V2, V3, LOC, and TOA during perceptual switches averaged across four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across subjects.
Figure 5
 
The average fMRI signals in V1, V2, V3, LOC, and TOA during perceptual switches averaged across four subjects. Time point zero indicates the time of the subject's response. Error bars denote 1 SEM, calculated across subjects.
Figure 6
 
Histograms of horizontal and vertical eye positions after removing blinks and artifacts during the diamond and non-diamond percepts from two subjects (the upper row for one subject and the lower row for the other).
Figure 6
 
Histograms of horizontal and vertical eye positions after removing blinks and artifacts during the diamond and non-diamond percepts from two subjects (the upper row for one subject and the lower row for the other).
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×