The purpose of this experiment was twofold. First, we wanted to know whether the better illusory discrimination was limited to stereo displays. To this end, binocular disparity in
Experiment 5 was reduced to zero in this experiment (while still using the red-green filters), and the amodal condition was maintained by closing the “mouth” of each inducer.
Second, we had observed that in the interfering conditions, although the illusory and amodal discriminations were both comparably worsened by the lines, the amodal shape was behind the four lines whereas the illusory shape and the four lines were co-planar. Hence, the interference in the amodal condition might be underestimated. To test this, we created a fifth condition, illusory + lines2 (
Figure 9), by putting the four interfering lines stereoscopically in front of the illusory shape. The disparity between these lines and the illusory shape was the same as that between the lines and the occluded shape before. In this way, we could test whether the interference was reduced when the four lines and the illusory shape were not co-planar.
Sixteen fresh observers participated with counterbalancing in the five conditions: illusory, amodal, illusory with lines, amodal with lines, and illusory with lines2. Feedback was provided per trial.
ANOVA of the first four conditions yielded results similar to the experiments before. The main effect of inducer rotation was significant, F(9, 135) = 355.58, p ≪ .001. The main effect of occlusion was significant, F(1, 15) = 23.19, p = .00022. The main effect of line interference was significant, F(1, 15) = 23.97, p = .00019. The interaction between line interference and inducer rotation was significant, F(9, 135) = 3.20, p = .0015. Finally, the interaction between occlusion and line interference was also significant, F(1, 15) = 4.94, p = .042. With the interference, discrimination worsened more for the illusory (78% to 72% correct) than for the amodal conditions (73% to 71% correct).
The last result indicated that the interfering lines impacted the illusory more than amodal shapes. This is probably because, although an amodal shape and the interfering lines had the same disparity of zero, perceptually the amodal shape by definition was behind the plane of the interfering lines. To test this conjecture more directly, we analyzed the three conditions when an illusory shape had no interfering lines, had the lines in the same plane, or had the lines in front. ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of the line conditions,
F(2, 30) = 11.07,
p = .00025. The main effect of inducer rotation was significant,
F(9, 135) = 344.05,
p ≪ .001. The interaction was also significant,
F(18, 270) = 1.84,
p = .021. A closer look revealed that the interfering lines being in a different plane had a smaller impact (76% correct) than being co-planar (72% correct),
F(1, 15) = 7.68,
p = .014. Yet, lines of different depth still interfered as compared with no lines (76% vs. 78% correct),
F(1, 15) = 4.42,
p = .053.
Figure 10 shows the results.
Finally, we combined data from the first four conditions in this experiment (
n = 16) and
Experiment 2 (
n = 11) to check whether stereo manipulation was any different from closing inducer “mouths” when stereo disparity was zero. The main difference between the two experiments was non-zero vs. zero binocular disparity. To our surprise, no difference whatsoever was found with the stereo disparity manipulation,
F(1, 25) = .003 ≪ 1. Apparently, the perceived occlusion relationship was primarily responsible for the difference between illusory and amodal discrimination, no matter if the occlusion relationship was created stereoscopically or pictorially.
The other effects remained: the main effect of occlusion was significant, F(1, 25) = 27.19, p ≪ .001. The main effect of line interference was significant, F(1, 25) = 27.99, p ≪ .001. The main effect of inducer rotation was highly significant, F(9, 225) = 457.39, p ≪ .001. The interaction between line interference and inducer rotation was also significant, F(9, 225) = 5.79, p ≪ .001.