We suggest the following hypothetical scenario for the neural mechanisms of subjective contour completion in order to account for the two effects: global alignment and local feature-spacing. The line ends within each inducer elicit responses in end-stopped cells in primary visual cortex V1 (Heitger, Rosenthaler, von der Heydt, Peterhans, & Kübler,
1992). These cells generate an occlusion signal which is transferred to several extrastriate areas (Mendola, Dale, Fischl, Liu, & Tootell,
1999), specifically to neurons in a cortical area termed the lateral occipital complex (LOC; Malach et al.,
1995). LOC neurons have been shown to be sensitive to the presence of salient regions, e.g., to contiguous parts of an image that belong to the same surface (Stanley & Rubin,
2003). Salient regions or surfaces are inferred from the proper geometrical alignment of local cues signaling occlusion; thus, the responses of LOC neurons to salient regions are important because they allow us to differentiate between aligned and outward pointing stimuli. We assume that proportional to their response, LOC neurons also send stronger feedback signals to early visual areas V2 and V1 for aligned than for outward pointing stimuli (Stanley & Rubin,
2003), and in turn the subjective contour signal in early visual cortex including V1 (Grosof, Shapley, & Hawken,
1993; Peterhans & von der Heydt,
1989) will be amplified for aligned stimulus configurations (Maertens & Pollman,
2005). Probe localization is likely to depend on signals in retinotopically organized visual areas because there the dot and the subjective contour evoke spatially discriminable neural responses. To account for the differences in interpolation accuracy between the two types of two-line Varin figures, there are at least two explanations. It is possible that the more closely spaced lines already evoke stronger responses in V1 because they might stimulate end-stopped neurons at a close to optimal sampling rate. Then these stronger responses are simply inherited by LOC and fed back to V1. Alternatively, the difference may arise first in responses of neurons in LOC. LOC neurons may learn to respond better to closely spaced Varin inducers because the narrow spacing has proven (by experience) to be a more reliable signal for occlusion than the wider one. Of course the hypothesized circuit is overly simplified and presumably involves a number of intermediate neural populations as well. But our major purpose here was to provide a sketch of cortical machinery that can account for both the effect of global geometrical alignment (LOC) and the effect of local geometry of line terminations (V1).