Most studies investigating occluded objects looked into the way the visual system completes occluded objects (Liinasuo, Kojo, Häkkinen, & Rovamo,
2004; Rensink & Enns,
1998; Ringach & Shapley,
1996; van Lier, van der Helm, & Leeuwenberg,
1995) or at the temporal aspects of completion (Bruno, Bertamini, & Domini,
1997; Guttman, Sekuler, & Kellman,
2003; Sekuler & Palmer,
1992; Shore & Enns,
1997). As far as we know, the only study that investigates which part of the occluded object is fixated is the study by Vishwanath, Kowler, and Feldman (
2000). They asked subjects to look at partly occluded triangles “as a whole.” If the completed shape of the triangle is taken into account by the visual system, subjects will look at the center of mass (COM) of the completed object, as when viewing non-occluded stimuli (Findlay,
1982; He & Kowler,
1989; Henderson,
1993; Kowler & Blaser,
1995; McGowan, Kowler, Sharma, & Chubb,
1998; Vishwanath & Kowler,
2003). In contrast, subjects looked at the center of mass of the visible fragment (COMvis) of the triangles. This suggests that even though information about the occluded object part is present in the visual system, this information is not used when making saccades towards that object. However, in the study of Vishwanath et al., only the first fixations on the object were investigated. Subjects started a trial by fixating a point located eccentrically from the target stimulus. Thus, information for the first saccade and the first fixation position were gathered from the periphery, which could have made it difficult to determine the completed shape of a partly occluded stimulus. This might have caused the first fixation to deviate from the center of mass of the completed stimulus. Once the eye has landed on the stimulus, more accurate information about the target stimulus will be available. Therefore, information about the completed shape of the stimulus might become apparent in the second fixation, which then might land around the COM.