Figure 3 shows results for all four participants. Notably, participants performed very near veridical in the baseline condition. This condition serves as a critical baseline for the two conditions in which an attentional shift is necessary. In the two remaining conditions, we found a strong and robust effect of cueing condition on the latency between the actual and reported onset times. Latencies for a shift with a peripheral cue were around 140 ms, and a latency of around 240 ms was obtained with a central cue. These values are in good agreement with the range of values reported in earlier studies using exogenous and endogenous cueing paradigms.
It is interesting to note that with the possible exception of S3, participants were very accurate at reporting the time on the cued clock when they were informed in advance of the to-be-attended location. This indicates that the task was not perceptually challenging and that the latencies measured were accurate estimates of the time that attention arrived on a particular object or location (in this case, the relevant clock). As an aside, it is also interesting to note that the baseline condition is similar to conditions previously observed to generate a flash lag effect (Patel et al.,
2000), yet we observed little or no lag in the reported times.
We obtained strikingly similar estimates of shift times in all four participants, particularly in the exogenously cued condition where participants fell within a 23-ms range. Because the response variable in the current experiment is nearly continuous, much more precise data can be acquired in far fewer trials than when using performance measures in orienting paradigms or discrete responses in attentional gating experiments.
Another advantage of the stimulus, and at the same time an interesting avenue for further research, is that the current experimental design can be easily adapted to an attentional tracking experiment, allowing for a comparison of estimates of the speed of attention derived from attentional tracking experiments (Horowitz, Holcombe, Wolfe, Arsenio, & DiMase,
2004; Verstraten, Cavanagh, & Labianca,
2000). Horowitz et al., for example, rather than attempting to measure the duration of single shifts of attention, measured the pace at which observers could make successive shifts in a predictable order. Interestingly, they found relatively slow rates: 200–250 ms during attentional pursuit (most analogous to exogenous cueing) and 300–500 ms for what they call attentional saccades (in the absence of an exogenous guide). Although these values lie far above results from earlier literature and those of the present study, they clearly show a similar pattern.
We believe that the method presented here has great potential for studying the speed of visual attention. A refinement of a century-old technique, the method provides precise and robust results after even a small number of trials. It avoids relying on any high-level processes such as character identification and is flexible enough to be comparable to the established paradigms for measuring the speed of attention.