Each experimental session consisted of three phases. In the first phase (pre-adaptation), the baseline behavior was ascertained by having the subject perform 60 trials (10 each of the single saccade conditions, 15 each of the double-saccade conditions), randomly intermixed. We then initiated the adaptation phase, in which the amplitude of single saccades to target B was altered by applying McLaughlin's intra-saccadic target step paradigm (McLaughlin,
1967). In this paradigm, the subject is asked to make a saccadic eye movement toward a visual target; while the eyes are moving at high speed, the target is displaced. This is repeated over and over. Initially, the subject's eyes land around the initial location of the target, and the target is eventually acquired by a secondary (corrective) saccade. However, over time the primary saccade is modified, so that after a while the subjects aim their eyes directly at (or close to) the final target location. The relationship between the visual and motor vectors is thus modified. Importantly, several lines of evidence (reviewed in the
Discussion section) indicate that the dissociation likely occurs not at the level of the conversion of the visual vector into a goal vector, but rather in the conversion of the goal vector into a movement vector. It is thus
fM() that is modified by this paradigm. The duration of this phase varied from session to session, depending on how quickly saccadic adaptation was induced (i.e., the amplitude of
changed). For amplitude reductions, we initially stepped back target B by 20% of the distance between B and FP. This percentage was then increased to 30%, and in some subjects up to 35%. Since
and
are not very far apart, we typically counteracted the tendency of adaptation to spread to
(Alahyane, Devauchelle, Salemme, & Pélisson,
2008; Alahyane et al.,
2007; Albano,
1996; Collins, Doré-Mazars, & Lappe,
2007; Deubel,
1987; Frens & van Opstal,
1994; Kojima, Iwamoto, & Yoshida,
2005; Miller, Anstis, & Templeton,
1981; Noto, Watanabe, & Fuchs,
1999; Semmlow, Gauthier, & Vercher,
1989; Straube, Fuchs, Usher, & Robinson,
1997; Wallman & Fuchs,
1998) by intra-saccadically stepping target A further away from FP during trials that required single saccades to A. This was sufficient to considerably limit (although not to abolish) changes in
. For amplitude increases, we initially stepped forward target B by 20% of the distance between B and FP. This percentage was then increased to 30%. Since in our setup gain increases were harder to induce than gain decreases (Deubel,
1987; Deubel, Wolf, & Hauske,
1986; Miller et al.,
1981), we never found it necessary or useful to push any further. In this case, target A was not intra-saccadically displaced during single saccades to it, since we did not find it to be necessary. During the adaptation phase, single saccades to target B were considerably more frequent (70% for amplitude decreases, 80% for amplitude increases) than the other trial types (which had equal frequency). This phase was terminated once an acceptable level of adaptation was reached. By default, this occurred after 150 adaptation trials, but the experimenter could either shorten this phase (but to no less than 120 adaptation trials) if an asymptote had been reached, or lengthen it (but to no more than 200 adaptation trials), if adaptation was proceeding slowly. The post-adaptation phase was similar to the pre-adaptation phase, with the only differences being that it encompassed twice as many trials (120) and that the intra-saccadic steps applied during the adaptation phase were continued (thus maintaining the adaptation).