The actual matches produced in response to each of the four sets of matching instructions are plotted in
Figure 12. For each subject and condition, the plots have been fit with either a linear or a parabolic regression model on the basis of the following statistical procedure. First, a parabolic (i.e., second-order polynomial) model was forced on the data by regressing the matches from each of the four conditions against two predictor variables: the target ring luminance and the
square of the target ring luminance (both in log units). A linear model was adopted in place of the parabolic model if the second variable could be removed without producing a statistically significant decrease in the amount of variance in the match disk settings accounted for by the regression model. For one analysis only (AH, brightness matching), neither predictor variable was significant. The results from that condition have been fit with a parabolic model in
Figure 12. This procedure resulted, for both observers, in fitting the brightness and one-spotlight lightness matches with a parabolic model and the brightness contrast and two-spotlight lightness matches with a linear model.
The parabolic model is parameterized by three regression coefficients: the intercept (b 0), slope (b 1), and curvature (b 2) of the second-order polynomial model. The results of the parabolic regression analyses are as follows: Brightness (AH: b 0 = 0.505 ± 0.003, p < 0.001; b 1 = 0.002 ± 0.015, p = 0.916; b 2 = 0.102 ± 0.080; r 2 = 0.457; MER: b 0 = 0.500 ± 0.011, p < 0.001; b 1 = 0.265 ± 0.051, p = 0.015; b 2 = 0.886 ± 0.270, p = 0.046; r 2 = 0.901); brightness contrast (AH: b 0 = 0.422 ± 0.034, p = 0.001; b 1 = −1.065 ± 0.152, p = 0.006; b 2 = −0.730 ± 0.805; r 2 = 0.960; MER: b 0 = 0.444 ± 0.010, p < 0.001; b 1 = −0.790 ± 0.045, p < 0.001; b 2 = −0.069 ± 0.241, p = 0.794; r 2 = 0.994); one-spotlight lightness (AH: b 0 = 0.497 ± 0.004, p < 0.001; b 1 = 0.084 ± 0.020, p = 0.025; b 2 = 0.331 ± 0.107, p = 0.053; r 2 = 0.856; MER: b 0 = 0.491 ± 0.007, p < 0.001; b 1 = 0.254 ± 0.030, p = 0.003; b 2 = 0.865 ± 0.158, p = 0.012; r 2 = 0.961); two-spotlight lightness (AH: b 0 = 0.466 ± 0.039, p = 0.001; b 1 = −0.854 ± 0.175, p = 0.016; b 2 = −1.023 ± 0.929, p = 0.351; r 2 = 0.913; MER: b 0 = 0.449 ± 0.012, p < 0.001; b 1 = −0.776 ± 0.052, p = 0.001; b 2 = 0.033 ± 0.278, p = 0.912; r 2 = 0.992).
The linear (reduced) model is parameterized by two regression coefficients only: the intercept (b 0) and slope (b 1). The results of the linear regression analyses, where appropriate, are: brightness contrast (AH: b 0 = 0.399 ± 0.022, p < 0.001; b 1 = 0.975 ± 0.112, p = 0.001; r 2 = 0.950; MER: b 0 = 0.442 ± 0.006, p < 0.001; b 1 = −0.782 ± 0.030, p < 0.001; r 2 = 0.994); two-spotlight lightness (AH: b 0 = 0.434 ± 0.027, p = 0.001; b 1 = −0.728 ± 0.136, p = 0.006; r 2 = 0.877; MER: b 0 = 0.450 ± 0.007, p < 0.001; b 1 = −0.780 ± 0.034, p < 0.001; r 2 = 0.992).
In two of the four conditions—brightness and brightness contrast—the matching results from observer AH conformed to the ideal observer models. No statistically significant curvature was found in her matching plots in either of these conditions; and the slopes of her plots were not significantly different from 0 in the brightness condition and −1 in the brightness contrast condition.
For the other observer in the brightness and brightness contrast conditions, and for both observers in the two lightness conditions, the results conformed to one of two other quantitative patterns. Either (1) the plot was a straight line having a slope of about −0.7 (contrast, two-spotlight lightness), as was also found in Rudd and Zemach's (
2004) naive matching study employing Wallach stimuli; or (2) the matching plots were parabolic (brightness, one-spotlight lightness conditions) and exhibited a contrast effect at low target ring luminances and assimilation at high target ring luminances. The latter pattern is the same as that found by Bressan and Actis-Grosso (
2001) for lightness matches performed with double-increment stimuli.
The main take-home message of Experiment 2 is that the matches made with DAR stimuli can depend on the subject's interpretation of the stimulus, as shown earlier by Arend and Spehar. Thus, one should be cautious in interpreting the results of matching experiments as evidence for a theory of lightness perception unless it is clear what perceptual dimensions of the stimulus are being attended to and matched by the observers. Furthermore, lightness matches are not always independent of the surround luminance in the case of double-increment stimuli. Contrary to the predictions of Anchoring Theory, the effect of manipulating the luminance of the lower luminance surround in a double-increment stimulus depends on the assumptions that the observer makes about the nature of the illumination. Overall, ideal observer models provide a better account of the results of Experiment 2 than does anchoring to the highest luminance.
It is worth noting in this regard that in at least one earlier study that found
no effect of surround luminance on the matches made with double-increment stimuli (Jacobsen & Gilchrist,
1988), observers were explicitly told to match the stimuli on
brightness when the target was an increment relative to its surround luminance. Despite this fact, the results of Jacobsen and Gilchrist's study have subsequently been cited (erroneously) as evidence in favor of highest luminance anchoring in
lightness perception (Gilchrist,
2006; Gilchrist et al.,
1999).
Recall that the naive matching results obtained by Rudd and Zemach (
2005) with the same physical stimuli used in Experiment 2 were highly variable (see
Figures 9 and
10). Nevertheless, for all three of their observers, the naive matches lay in between the results obtained here with the two different sets of lightness matching instructions (or, alternatively, the brightness and brightness contrast instructions). This suggests that the naive matches in our 2005 study may have been based on a compromise between alternative interpretations of the disk appearance dimension to be matched.
The regularities seen across observers in terms of their patterns of errors or deviations from the ideal observer predictions are also worth emphasizing. The results from the two
lightness matching conditions of Experiment 2 exhibit regularities that mirror findings obtained in our 2004 study with Wallach stimuli and with double-decrement stimuli in Experiment 1 of the present study. First, roughly linear matching plots having slopes of about −0.7 were found in both the two-spotlight matching condition of Experiment 2 and in our earlier study employing Wallach stimuli having surround rings of the same size. The fact that strong contrast effects were obtained both with Wallach stimuli in our 2004 study and with two-spotlight instructions here is consistent with Wallach's interpretation of his own results as an attempt on the part of the observers to achieve lightness constancy under the assumption that the two sides of the display are lit by separate illuminants. Nevertheless, in both our 2004 study and in the two-spotlight condition of Experiment 2, our observers failed to make the exact ratio matches predicted for an ideal observer under two-spotlight lightness matching instructions, as indicated by the fact that the matching plot slopes were about −0.7, rather than −1. It seems likely that that this deviation from exact ratio matching is due to the small size of the 0.35 deg surround used in both experiments. The plots obtained with 0.35 deg Wallach stimuli in our 2004 study were about −0.7, but the slopes tended to decrease toward −1 as the ring size was increased in that study (Rudd & Zemach,
2004). Further work is required to determine whether a perfect ratio match would hold for double-increment and two-spotlight instructions if the surround size were increased.
Second, parabolic matching functions exhibiting both contrast and assimilation effects over different ranges of the surround luminance ranges were obtained both for naive matches made with double-decrement stimuli in Experiment 1 of the present study and for one-spotlight lightness matches made with double-increment stimuli in Experiment 2. In the next section, a neural model that accounts for this regularity is proposed. The model also accounts for the rest of the results discussed to this point, including both the parabolic and approximately linear matching plots, and their dependence on luminance polarity, surround size, and matching instructions. The model is able to account both for the aspects of the data that approximate ideal observer behavior (i.e., constancy) and for the pattern of errors. By incorporating attentional feedback to adjust the model parameters in a task-specific way, the model is able to fit the results of both lightness matching conditions of Experiment 2 in the context of a single unified neural theory.