Despite the well-documented effects of aging on motion perception, it still remains unclear why motion perception declines with aging. Although aging is accompanied by changes in optical factors (e.g., Sloane, Owsley, & Alvarez,
1988; Weale,
1961,
1986,
1988; Winn, Whitaker, Elliott, & Phillips,
1994), these have been shown to be insufficient to account for age-related declines in motion perception (Ball & Sekuler,
1986; Betts, Sekuler, & Bennett,
2009; Norman et al.,
2003). Therefore, changes at the neuronal level are likely to be the cause of these effects. Anatomical studies of the retinogeniculostriate pathway in primates found that the number, volume, and density of neurons do not change significantly with normal aging (Ahmad & Spear,
1993; Kim, Pier, & Spear,
1997; Peters, Nigro, & McNally,
1997; Spear,
1993). However, neurophysiological studies in senescent monkeys and cats have revealed significant declines in neuronal function. For example, senescent neurons in the striate cortex exhibit higher excitability, reduced orientation and direction selectivity, increased levels of neuronal noise, and reduced signal-to-noise ratios (Hua et al.,
2006; Schmolesky, Wang, Pu, & Leventhal,
2000; Zhang et al.,
2008). Extrastriate neurons in the middle temporal area (MT), which are especially important for motion perception (e.g., Maunsell & Van Essen,
1983; Tootell et al.,
1995), show not only increased noise and reduced direction selectivity (Liang et al.,
2008; Yang et al.,
2008) but also have lower preferred speeds and broader speed tuning functions (Yang, Zhang et al.,
2009). These functional changes may in part be mediated by decreased levels of intracortical inhibition (Hua, Kao, Sun, Li, & Zhou,
2008; Leventhal, Wang, Pu, Zhou, & Ma,
2003). Given the importance of V1 and MT neurons in human motion perception (Clifford & Ibbotson,
2002), alterations in neuronal function in aging humans may be similar to the changes observed in aging monkeys as described above.