For example, areas V1 and middle temporal (MT) contain units that are tuned for linear (i.e., translational) motion (Albright,
1984; Hubel & Wiesel,
1962; Livingstone & Hubel,
1988; Maunsell & Van Essen,
1983a,
1983b), while the dorsal medial superior temporal area (MSTd), located downstream from V1 and MT in the visual pathway, contains units selective for the processing of complex motion stimuli such as spirals (which are composed of a wide range of motion directions and contain speed gradients) as well as linear motion (Graziano, Andersen, & Snowden,
1994; Saito et al.,
1986; Tanaka, Fukada, & Saito,
1989; Tanaka et al.,
1986). In MT approximately 75% of neurons are tuned for linear motion with a preference for unidirectional motion away from the fovea rather than toward it (Albright,
1989; Tanaka et al.,
1986); while in MSTd approximately 50% are similarly tuned (Tanaka et al.,
1986). Besides a population of translation motion specific units, it has been reported that in MSTd expansion-tuned cells outnumber contraction cells by a ratio of 2:1 and rotation cells by a ratio of 3:1 (Duffy & Wurtz,
1991a,
1991b; Graziano et al.,
1994; Saito et al.,
1986; Tanaka et al.,
1989). Magnetoenchephalography (MEG) studies in humans, recorded over the homologue of MT/MST, also indicate a preference for expanding stimuli compared to other motions (Holliday & Meese,
2005). Holliday and Meese (
2005) hypothesized that the relative numbers, or sensitivities, of differently tuned neuron groups are relative to the magnitude of their evoked magnetic responses; since expanding visual stimuli produced the greatest magnetic response and findings of the greater proportioned expansion-tuned units had been published, they suggested that the evoked magnetic responses are larger for motion stimuli that activate a larger pool of neurons. This bias may be related to the ecologic value of expansion relative to other spiral motions for primates, i.e., expanding patterns could indicate a potential collision (Franconeri & Simons,
2003; Gonzalez et al.,
1988; Hassenstein & Hustert,
1999; Lee et al.,
1976; Regan & Beverly,
1978; Regan & Hamstra,
1993; von Muhlenen & Lleras,
2007; Wang & Frost,
1992) and are common during forward locomotion since they are important in computing direction of heading (Crowell & Banks,
1993; T. C. Freeman, Harris, & Tyler,
1994; Gibson,
1950; Warren & Hannon,
1988). Moreover, evidence from motion transparency depth rivalry experiments has indicated that the relevance of a stimulus influences rivalry (Chopin & Mamassian,
2011; Maruya, Yang, & Blake,
2007). This suggests that the ecologically relevant expansion stimulus, which has influenced the perception of human observers during tasks such as visual search (Takeuchi,
1996) and speed illusions (Geesaman & Qian,
1996), may also influence dominance durations during binocular rivalry (Parker & Alais,
2007; Wade et al.,
1984).