Visual working memory (VWM) stores a very limited amount of visual information (e.g., Baddeley,
2010; Hollingworth,
2006; Jiang, Makovski, & Shim,
2009; Xu & Chun,
2006; Zhang & Luck,
2008); for example, statistical estimation suggests that it holds a maximum of about three to four simple objects (Baddeley,
2012; Cowan,
2001; Fukuda, Awh, & Vogel,
2010). However, VWM has incredible relationships with many important, high-level human activities (e.g., Cowan et al.,
2005; Hollingworth, Richard, & Luck,
2008; Hyun & Luck,
2007; Issen & Knill,
2012; Woodman, Luck, & Schall,
2007), and VWM status even offers valuable information on clinical diagnosis (e.g., Della Sala, Parra, Fabi, Luzzi, & Abrahams,
2012; Lee et al.,
2010; Leonard et al.,
2012; McCabe, Smith, & Parks,
2007; Park, Puschel, Sauter, Rentsch, & Hell,
2003). Consequently, researchers from various fields are interested in VWM. Using behavioral, event-related potentials (ERP), magnetoencephalography, and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) methods, investigators have examined multiple aspects of VWM, such as capacity, representation resolution, and information filtering in children, young adults, old adults, and people with neurological disorders (e.g., Bays & Husain,
2008; Cowan, Morey, AuBuchon, Zwilling, & Gilchrist,
2010; Grimault et al.,
2009; Jost, Bryck, Vogel, & Mayr,
2011; Lee et al.,
2010; Lopez-Crespo, Daza, & Mendez-Lopez,
2012; van den Berg, Shin, Chou, George, & Ma,
2012; Xu & Chun,
2006).