There has been growing interest in the mechanisms for the computation of approximate visual numerosity using sparse textures, such as those illustrated in
Figure 1, in adult humans (Burr & Ross,
2008; Dakin, Tibber, Greenwood, Kingdom, & Morgan,
2011; Durgin,
1995,
2008; Ross & Burr,
2010), in infants (Xu & Spelke,
2000), in animals (Brannon, Wusthoff, Gallistel, & Gibbon,
2001; Gallistel,
1989; Leslie, Gelman, & Gallistel,
2008), in functional magnetic resonance imaging studies (Piazza, Pinel, Le Bihan, & Dehaene,
2007), and even in single neurons for small (<10) numbers of objects (Nieder,
2005). Psychophysical experiments show that two regular shapes, such as circles, can be discriminated on the basis of the number of elementary objects they contain and that the threshold Weber fraction for this ability in adult humans of normal ability is in the region of 20% (Ross & Burr,
2010). What is less clear is how this is done. It is usually assumed, because of the speed with which large (>10) numbers can be discriminated and because of the adherence to Weber's law, that the computation is not done by direct enumeration (Ross & Burr,
2010). But if not by enumeration, how? The way in which large numbers are measured in physics may provide a clue. In physics, numbers are dimensionless and are thereby distinguished from constants, which are measured along dimensions such as length (m) and temperature (K). For example, Avogadro's number (∼6.022 × 10
23) represents the number of elementary entities of a substance in 1 mole of that substance. Clearly, this very large number was not obtained by enumeration. Instead, it was established in the first instance as a ratio of the charge of 1 mole of electrons to the elementary charge. By suitable choice of two measurements having the same dimension, a dimensionless number can always be obtained as a ratio.
1