A reference orientation direction is fundamental to perception and action. Knowing one's orientation and the orientation of surrounding objects in relation to gravity affects the ability to maintain postural stability (Kluzik, Horak, & Peterka,
2005; Wade & Jones,
1997) as well as the ability to identify (Dyde, Jenkin, & Harris,
2006), predict the behavior of (Barnett-Cowan, Fleming, Singh, & Bülthoff,
2011) and interact with, surrounding objects (McIntyre, Zago, Berthoz, & Lacquaniti,
2001). Gravity is ideally suited as a reference direction for orientation because it is universally available. However, the senses provide different types of information about the direction of gravity. Thus optimal performance requires integrating orientation cues from multiple senses. Several studies in the perceptual literature suggest that the orientation which best enables the brain to reconstruct the three-dimensional structure of objects, rather than being aligned with gravity, is biased towards the observer's left (Jenkin, Dyde, Jenkin, Harris, & Howard,
2003; Jenkin, Jenkin, Dyde, & Harris,
2004; Mamassian & Goutcher,
2001; McManus, Buckman, & Woolley,
2004; Metzger,
1975; Sun & Perona,
1998). Howard, Bergström, & Ohmi (
1990) found that obtaining shape from shading is predominantly performed in a headcentric frame of reference, and while trends of a leftward bias relative to the head were found regardless of head tilt they did not reach significance. Results from Jenkin et al. (
2003) and Jenkin et al. (
2004) also show a small trend for preferred lighting to be to the left relative to the head when oriented upright (Jenkin et al.,
2003: see figure 4a; Jenkin et al.,
2004: 5.7° to the left, figure 3) and even when upside down (Jenkin et al.,
2003, see figure 4b). This latter observation is crucial because it indicates that the bias is in a constant direction relative to the head.