The activity of MT neurons has been associated with the perception of motion coherence (e.g., Britten, Shadlen, Newsome, & Movshon,
1992; Britten, Shadlen, Newsome, & Movshon,
1993; Cohen & Newsome,
2009). Most MT neurons respond linearly with coherence (Britten et al.,
1993), which is consistent with the linear behavior of the perceptual response function that we found for high coherences for the No surround condition (
Figure 3). Some other MT neurons respond with expansive nonlinearities, which is consistent with the pedestal effect, but about the same number respond with compressive nonlinearities (Britten et al.,
1993). While it is encouraging to find qualitative agreement of the psychophysical response functions with the physiological responses of single MT neurons, we do not expect a quantitative agreement in the exact non-linear shape of the response functions for several reasons. First, stimulus conditions are not the same. While we used brief stimuli, the effect of coherence for a large range of coherences on MT neurons has been tested only using long stimuli and their activity averaged across long periods (Britten et al.,
1993). Second, while we used a discrimination threshold as the unit of psychophysical response assuming additive noise, this is not necessarily the case for neural response. For long stimuli, for example, the noise of MT neurons increases with the response (Britten et al.,
1993; Snowden, Treue, & Andersen,
1992). It is possible, however, that the noise becomes more additive for brief stimuli (Müller, Metha, Krauskopf, & Lennie,
2001). Third, one needs to consider that perception depends not on the activity of a single neuron, but on the activity of a population of neurons (Cohen & Newsome,
2009; Sanborn & Dayan,
2011) and how this activity is decoded (Gold & Ding,
2013).