To date, there are no reports of neural circuitries that qualify for both criteria. Yet there are reports of neural structures in V1 and V2, whose functions are still called a mystery (Allman, Miezin, & McGuinness,
1985; Anzai, Peng, & Van Essen,
2007) but whose properties suggest a role in the perception of slant. In the visual cortex of carnivores and primates, neurons selective for the orientation of visual edges are organized in orientation columns, which are vertical arrays of neurons that prefer the same orientation (Hubel & Wiesel,
1977; Blasdel & Salama,
1986; Bonhoeffer & Grinvald,
1991; Ohki, Chung, Ch'ng, Kara, & Reid,
2005; Ohki, Chung, Kara, Hübener, Bonhoeffer, & Reid,
2006). An important feature of V1 connections is the plexus of long-range horizontal connections most prominent in Layers 2 and 3, which enable neurons to integrate inputs from an area of cortex representing an area of visual field that is much larger than their classical receptive fields. Connections beyond their classical receptive fields are a prerequisite for neurons coding for slant. Although the extent and orientation dependence of long-range horizontal connections have been found to match the properties of salient contours and the geometry of natural scene contours (Gilbert & Wiesel,
1989; Li & Gilbert,
2002; Stettler, Das, Bennett, & Gilbert,
2002; Gilbert & Li,
2012), the function performed by this general feature of the cortical architecture remains obscure (Muir et al.,
2011). Neurons selective for the orientation of visual edges at different retinal locations in combination with interneuron long-range connections form a structure that seems the natural candidate for the coding of slant. Cells in V2 responsive to combinations of orientations have been attributed the function of encoding contours and providing cues for surface segmentation (Anzai, Peng, & Van Essen,
2007). Showing optimal responses to different orientations (
α) in different parts of their receptive fields (
β) implies, however, that such cells may carry the required signal for the coding of 3D slant. Another component essential for the coding of slant is a structure signaling that detected combinations of orientations belong to a common shape. V4 and the lateral occipital cortex (LOC) are likely candidates because these areas play an important role in human object recognition (Kourtzi & Kanwisher,
2001; Silson et al.,
2013). Recent high-density electroencephalogram recordings show that the LOC is critically involved in perceptual decisions about object shape (Ales, Appelbaum, Cottereau, & Norcia,
2013). V4 cells having 3D orientation tuning (i.e., tuning for specific slants) may also be involved in signaling a common shape to V2 or V1 orientation detectors (Hinkle & Connor,
2002). Cells responsive to linear perspective have been reported in the caudal intraparietal sulcus (CIP; Tsutsui, Jiang, Yara, Sakata, & Taira,
2001; Tsutsui, Taira, & Sakata,
2005). These neurons were also sensitive to binocular disparity and thus probably involved in the integration of both types of information. Nevertheless, the authors did not look for perspective-related activity in the occipital cortex as they did for binocular disparity. In conclusion, a number of neural structures in V1, V2, V4, LOC, and CIP show properties that are essential for the detection and coding of 3D slant. However, the neural activity of most structures has never been tested with specific slant stimuli. The present study provides stimuli and experimental techniques that enable the isolation of assumption-based slant from slant induced by screen-related cues. Computations and psychophysical results show that the combination of perspective-induced convergence (
α) and angular separation (
β) of lines or contours carries the information for 3D slant. Further studies on the neural processing of slant will provide important insight into networks mediating perspective cues for perception of the 3D world.