At least two theoretical frameworks bear on these fundamental questions. To begin, He and colleagues have put forth a body of work and an account of distance perception that does posit a very specific role for selection processes (He, Wu, Ooi, Yarbrough, & Wu,
2004; Ooi & He,
2007; J. Wu, He, & Ooi,
2008; B. Wu, Ooi, & He,
2004). Their account builds from the idea that the ground surface provides an important frame of reference for localizing objects (Gibson,
1950,
1979; Sedgwick,
1986; see also Bian, Braunstein, & Andersen,
2005,
2006). Specifically, they suggest that an accurate perception of distance depends on the acquisition of an accurate representation of the ground surface, which in turn depends on a
sequential surface integration process (SSIP). Because the nearby distance and depth cues are more reliable, an accurate representation of the immediate ground surface provides an important constraint on the development of a ground representation at further distances where the distance and depth cues are less reliable. Thus, by this account, information about the nearby ground surface provides a kind of anchor for the integration of information about farther patches of the ground surface. The results of several experiments have supported this idea. For example, targets were localized less accurately when viewed through an aperture that occluded the nearby ground plane and performance was better when observers were asked to scan from the near ground to the object than vice versa (B. Wu et al.,
2004). In addition, J. Wu et al. (
2008) have shown that if a task biases participants' initial attention toward distances lying beyond the immediate ground surface, the accuracy of distance judgments suffers, presumably because the crucial information from the immediate ground plane has not been selected or picked up. Eye movements were not monitored in any of these studies, and the account is agnostic about whether selection can be accomplished covertly or if overt shifts of attention are required. Nevertheless, while attention can be directed covertly, attention and the direction of gaze are normally dynamically coupled (e.g., Henderson,
1992; Henderson, Pollatsek, & Rayner,
1989; Hoffman & Subramaniam,
1995; Kowler, Anderson, Dosher, & Blaser,
1995; Shepherd, Findlay, & Hockey,
1986). Thus, the benefit of scanning from near to far would predict an important role for eye movements.