Experiments, originally performed by Arend and Reeves (
1986), indicate that illuminant differences between otherwise identical scenes (computer-simulated two-dimensional “Mondrians”) are readily visible even without reference surfaces available that provide conclusive cues. Furthermore, their results indicate that (some) observers can use cues to the illuminant color from reference surfaces to improve color constancy, while others find this task difficult to perform (Cornelissen & Brenner,
1995; Granzier, Vergne, & Gegenfurtner,
2013). These results suggest that illumination perception and color constancy are strongly linked. However, when it was explicitly tested to determine whether such a link exists, the results seem to indicate otherwise (Granzier, Brenner, & Smeets,
2009a; Granzier, Nijboer, Smeets, & Brenner,
2005; Granzier, Smeets, & Brenner,
2012). These results make the precise relationship between color constancy and illumination perception complex. The results of Arend and Reeves (
1986) introduced the interesting issue whether an observer can represent, simultaneously, the color of a surface and that of the light illuminating it (Arend,
1994; MacLeod,
2003; Mausfeld
1998). There is to date little experimental evidence for such multidimensional perceptual responses for chromatic scenes. The focus of the current investigation, however, is on daylight brightness and chromaticity variations and whether observers are able to use the brightness and chromaticity information to predict time of day and season or month. Thus, the term “illumination” in this paper only refers to its chromaticity and brightness (not distribution, flow, etc.) and we do not address, for instance, the angular distribution of the luminance.