We studied a relatively simple stimulus configuration, patterned after the one introduced by A. L. Gilchrist (
1980). This stimulus configuration was of interest because it can lead to large geometric effects (A. L. Gilchrist,
1980; Radonjić et al.,
2010) when the stimuli consist of real illuminated surfaces but is simple enough that key parameters can be varied parametrically. To facilitate our stimulus manipulations, we presented the stimuli as simulations on a computer-controlled stereo display. For reasons that are not clear, the size of the geometric effects in our experiments is smaller than found in the original A. L. Gilchrist (
1980) study and in its more recent replication (Radonjić et al.,
2010).
Figure 9 replots our data from
Experiment 1 for the largest illuminant change condition (
Figure 3D) along with data from two experiments from Radonjić et al. (
2010, data from their experiment 2, figure 6 and experiment 3, figure 8). Their experiment 2 (their figure 6, blue triangles in our
Figure 9) was similar to ours in design but was conducted with real illuminated papers. In addition, their background context planes were spatially homogeneous rather than articulated, they used a between-subjects rather than a within-subjects design, two probe tabs were visible in their stimuli in each trial, and they studied only one tab luminance. There are two salient differences between their data and ours. First, their geometric effect is considerably larger than ours. Second, their photometric effect goes in the opposite direction from ours. The fact that our stimuli were articulated and theirs were not does not explain these differences as articulation increases rather than decreases the geometric effect when the stimuli consist of real illuminated papers (Radonjić & Gilchrist,
2013). In their experiment 3, the probe tabs were spatially separated from the background context planes (see their figure 7). The data from this second experiment are much more similar to ours. Relative to their condition in which the probe tabs are adjacent to the background context planes, the geometric effect is reduced, and the photometric effect switches sign. Although the processes that mediate the differences between these experiments are not known, the similarity in the effect of changing from real to simulated surfaces and the effect of changing from adjacent to separated probe tabs is intriguing, as it suggests that both effects could be mediated by a common process that regulates the influence of the coplanar context surface on the lightness of the probe tabs. If so, the principles our experiments reveal may apply to this underlying process even if the stimulus factors that lead it to govern performance differ between stimuli consisting of real versus simulated surfaces. Understanding this more fully is an interesting direction for future research, not in small measure because it may provide clues as to why the effects we find here using computer simulations differ from those found previously using real illuminated surfaces for a fairly well-matched stimulus configuration. In turn, a better understanding of the key factors that must be incorporated into simulated scenes to have them evoke the same performance as scenes consisting of real illuminated surfaces would facilitate the study of larger effects using the parametric/computer display approach we have developed here and allow more powerful tests of the interaction between photometric and geometric context than we were able to obtain with our current methods.