Visual search has served as a powerful tool to investigate several aspects of visual stimulus processing under controlled laboratory conditions in both humans and monkeys (e.g., Bichot & Desimone,
2006; Davis & Palmer,
2004; Ipata, Gee, Gottlieb, Bisley, & Goldberg,
2006; Mirpour & Bisley,
2012b; Mort & Kennard,
2003; Muller & Krummenacher,
2006). For a long period, most visual search research in humans relied upon reaction time and error rate as measures of search performance using experiments where eye movements were either not studied (A. Treisman,
1993; A. M. Treisman & Gelade,
1980; Wolfe,
2007) or where brief stimuli were used to ensure that search performance was based on information obtained while the eye remained fixed at the center of gaze (Cameron, Tai, Eckstein, & Carrasco,
2004; Verghese,
2001). Similarly, in monkeys, experiments generally required the monkey to maintain the eye at the center of gaze or to foveate the target with its eyes as a response (e.g., Bichot, Rossi, & Desimone,
2005; Mirpour, Arcizet, Ong, & Bisley,
2009; Mirpour & Bisley,
2012b; Motter & Belky,
1998a), usually with the first saccade (e.g., McPeek & Keller,
2001; Schall & Thompson,
1999). In human subjects, there has also been an active research field exploring oculomotor behavior during more naturalistic visual search where the subjects are free to move their eyes as they please (e.g., Eckstein,
2011; Geisler & Chou,
1995; Rao, Zelinsky, Hayhoe, & Ballard,
2002; Tatler, Hayhoe, Land, & Ballard,
2011; Zelinsky,
1996; Zelinsky & Sheinberg,
1997). Similar analysis of eye movements in humans during reading and scene perception has revealed much about the mechanisms that regulate how humans move their eyes while processing complex visual information (reviewed in Rayner,
1998). In order to understand the neural mechanisms operating to guide eye movements during free-viewing visual search, recent studies have begun to report such data from monkeys. Analysis of eye-fixation patterns during free-viewing visual search has revealed a generally similar pattern of results relating search time and the number of fixations (Bisley, Ipata, Krishna, Gee, & Goldberg,
2009; Nothdurft, Pigarev, & Kastner,
2009). Analysis of eye-fixation patterns while free-viewing movie clips and natural images has also shown substantial similarity between human and monkey scanpaths, with a greater importance for low-level visual saliency information in monkeys (Berg, Boehnke, Marino, Munoz, & Itti,
2009; Einhauser, Kruse, Hoffmann, & Konig,
2006; Shepherd, Steckenfinger, Hasson, & Ghazanfar,
2010). Finally, physiological studies in monkeys from areas LIP and V4 (Gee, Ipata, & Goldberg,
2010; Ipata, Gee, Bisley, & Goldberg,
2009; Ipata, Gee, Goldberg, & Bisley,
2006; Ipata, Gee, Gottlieb, et al.,
2006) as well as the inferior temporal cortex (IT: Mruczek & Sheinberg,
2007; Sheinberg & Logothetis,
2001) have begun to elucidate the neural responses that lead to saccade-goal selection, target guidance, and target verification during free-viewing visual search.