Maljkovic and Nakayama (
1994) found that responses were facilitated when the target color up to five trials prior matched the target color in the current trial, regardless of intervening events, suggesting that the memory trace associated with the target color lasted beyond a single trial.
We used a similar approach to investigate the time course of sequential effects of target selection for action on curvature in the context of target color repetition. We examined whether a partial error in a single prior trial (up to 10 trials prior to the current trial) affected performance in the current trial. For each N-back, we first sorted trials according to whether the target color in the N-back trial matched the target in the current trial. Then, we determined whether the response in the N-back trial was a direct movement or a partial error. We calculated the match score for each N-back that matched the current target color as the curvature in trial N when the response in the N-back trial was a partial error minus the curvature in trial N when the response in the N-back trial was a direct movement. The nonmatch score was calculated the same way for trials in which the N-back target color did not match the current target color.
Earlier, we reported that trials following partial errors exhibited higher curvature than trials following direct movement only when the target color matched. If this same effect carries over multiple intervening trials, we would expect the match score to be positive, reflecting high curvature following partial errors in the N-back trial relative to direct movements in the N-back trial when the colors of the N-back trial and the current trial matched. We would also expect the nonmatch score to be near zero as previous trial performance would not affect performance in the current trial if the colors switched.
We conducted a 2 × 10 repeated-measures ANOVA with factors of score (match vs. nonmatch) and N-back (1–10). As expected, mean match score (.0109 ± .0025) was larger than mean nonmatch score (.0001 ± .0023), F(1, 15) = 8.32, p < 0.01. There was no main effect of N-back, F(9, 135) = 1.37, p > 0.1, but the interaction between score and N-back was marginally significant, F(9, 135) = 1.68, p = 0.1. We conducted separate one-way ANOVAs to determine the effect of N-back on match scores and nonmatch scores separately. There was a significant effect of N-back for match scores, F(9, 135) = 2,4, p < 0.01., with a significant linear trend, F(1, 15) = 12.86, p < 0.01, suggesting that match scores were decreasing in magnitude as the N-back got larger. There was no effect of N-back for nonmatch scores, F(9, 135) < 1.
A clear trend can be seen of a positive
match score that diminishes slowly over time in
Figure 3C, confirmed by the linear trend reported above. The match score is positive for the first five N-back trials while the nonmatch score is near zero (or negative) for the first five N-back trials. These data are suggestive of the notion that performance in a previous trial can affect performance in the current trial, even if there are several intervening trials, as long as the target-defining feature is shared between the two trials. Thus, the effect of selecting a nontarget object for action on subsequent behavior may be long-lasting, diminishing slowly over time.