The “attentional load” theory (Lavie,
2005) states that if attention is drawn by one stimulus, there is less attention available for another. If sustained attention increases the magnitude of sensory adaptation (Beck, Rees, Frith, & Lavie,
2001; Chaudhuri,
1990; Rezec, Krekelberg, & Dobkins,
2004; Taya, Adams, Graf, & Lavie,
2009), load theory predicts that distracting attention from an adapting stimulus will decrease its adapting effect. A key experiment for load theory (Rees, Frith, & Lavie,
1997) showed this to be true for the movement after-effect caused by large-field moving stimuli with a distracting stimulus placed foveally in its center. Furthermore, the same study showed that directing attention toward or away from a spatially-located visual target has large and robust effects on the blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) effects in the human primary visual cortex (Rees et al.,
1997; Schwartz et al.,
2005; Watanabe et al.,
2011), suggesting that sustained attention to a stimulus is equivalent to increasing its contrast (Ling & Carrasco,
2006).
However, there are problems with this reasoning. The first is the absence of a clear psychophysical linking hypothesis (Brindley,
1960) relating the level of the BOLD signal to the strength of adaptation. Presumably, the linking hypothesis is that the positive BOLD signal is correlated with increased neural activity and that increases in neural activity are positively correlated with adaptation. If this is so, attending to an adaptor should have an effect on adaptation equivalent to an increase in its contrast, but this was not demonstrated in the experiment (Rees et al.,
1997), which used only the same high-contrast (100%) adaptor in both attentional conditions. The strength of the motion after-effect saturates very rapidly as contrast is raised (Blake, Tadin, Sobel, Raissian, & Chong,
2006; Morgan, Chubb, & Solomon,
2011; Rezec et al.,
2004), so it is implausible that the effect of the high-contrast adaptor would be significantly modulated by attention. The linking hypothesis thus justifies skepticism, particularly since it involves the counterintuitive notion that attending to a stimulus over a period of time reduces its effective contrast.
The second problem is that the behavioral effect of attention in increasing adaptation is by no means clearly established. Some studies have found an effect (Beck et al.,
2001; Chaudhuri,
1990; Rezec et al.,
2004; Taya et al.,
2009), but others have found no effect (Morgan,
2011,
2012; Rees, Frith, & Lavie,
2001; Wohlgemuth,
1911), while yet others have found both positive and negative results in conceptual replications (Georgiades & Harris,
2002; Nishida & Ashida,
2000). A problem with most of the experiments is that they have used behavioral measures known to be susceptible to observer cognitive bias and expectation (see
Discussion).
Morgan, Dillenburger, Raphael, and Solomon (
2012) have shown that the standard method of single stimuli (MSS) for measuring the effects of adaptation cannot distinguish between genuine perceptual biases and a change in decisional criterion. Subjects can voluntarily shift their psychometric functions without changing their slope. The strategy here is to design a test procedure such that it is difficult for a subject to influence the effect of adaptation with a response or decisional bias, even if they know the expected direction of adaptation. As pointed out elsewhere (Garcia-Perez & Alcala-Quintana,
2012) a good start can be made in removing response bias by using 2AFC rather than MSS. The test stimulus can be added either to an adapted or an unadapted location, or even better to two differently-adapted locations. The 2AFC design can be refined by randomly interleaving different conditions of test such a change in adaptation state does not correspond to a simple change in decisional criterion.
This paper presents a proof-of-concept of the multicondition 2AFC design for measuring adaptation. We use the reduction in perceived velocity of a test stimulus after adapting to a stimulus moving in the same direction as the test (Thompson,
1981). The subject is adapted to two vertically-aligned moving grating patches moving in opposite directions, and tested in a two-alternative forced procedure (2AFC spatial) procedure with patches in which the physical speed difference is varied between the top and bottom patches (
Figure 1). The subject has to decide which is moving more quickly.