We have just considered correlations between different perceptual qualities and found that subjects can rate materials along the nine different qualities as if they were distinct dimensions. To what extent is the same true of different material classes? Are different material classes highly distinct, or do some classes correlate with one another? As we have already seen from the pattern of average ratings shown in
Figure 5, some material classes (e.g., wood and stone) do tend to correlate relatively well with one another across the nine perceptual qualities we tested, whereas others (e.g., metal and fabric) appear to be quite independent. In
Figure 8, we assess this in more detail by plotting the correlation matrix relating each material class to each other.
The correlation coefficients range from −0.3408 to 0.5815, indicating that the correlations between different material classes are generally relatively modest. The most strongly positively correlated material classes are stone and wood (r = 0.5815), followed by metal and leather (r = 0.4141). The correlation between stone and wood is fairly unsurprising: Of the material classes considered here, these are probably subjectively the most similar, both being naturally hard, often rough, brownish in color, and grained. Although, in everyday life, we would rarely confuse wood with stone, intuitively it seems that when compared with other classes (e.g., fabric, water, or glass), wood and stone are rather similar to one another. The correlation between metal and leather is somewhat more surprising: When asked verbally which classes are most similar to one another, none of the subjects reported these classes. At the same time, it is also important to note that correlation does not capture the absolute similarity in ratings between two classes of material; it simply measures the tendency for different perceptual qualities to increase or decrease in union. Paired sample t tests between the ratings for each perceptual quality for leather and metal materials reveals that the two classes were significantly different for six of the nine perceptual qualities: “glossiness” (t = −3.3698; p < 10−3), “transparency” (t = −3.3360; p < 10−3), “hardness” (t = −20.1050; p < 10−39), “coldness” (t = −11.8454; p < 10−21), “fragility” (t = 4.1446; p < 10−4), and “naturalness” (t = 4.9169; p < 10−6). Thus, correlations alone do not capture the true perceived similarities between different material classes. To evaluate this, we must consider the distribution of the samples in the 9-D feature space defined by the different perceptual qualities.