Figure 13 plots the mean probability estimates of perceived surface pigmentation as a function of global image orientation. For planar surfaces with low-frequency texture, a repeated-measures ANOVA on the arc-sine transformed data found no significant main effect of image orientation on perceived surface pigmentation,
F(2, 26) = 2.17,
p = 0.14. There was a significant main effect of texture orientation relative to shading on perceived pigmentation,
F(1, 13) = 19.22,
p < 0.001, but no interaction effect between texture and image orientation,
F(2, 26) = 0.32,
p = 0.73. For planar surfaces with high-frequency texture, another repeated-measures ANOVA on the arc-sine transformed data found no significant main effect of image orientation on perceived surface pigmentation,
F(2, 26) = 1.96,
p = 0.16. There was a significant main effect of texture orientation relative to shading on perceived pigmentation,
F(1, 13) = 35.71,
p < 0.00005, but no interaction effect between texture and image orientation,
F(2, 26) = 1.56,
p = 0.23.
For spherical surfaces with low-frequency texture, a repeated-measures ANOVA on arc-sine transformed probability estimates found a significant main effect of image orientation on perceived pigmentation, F(2, 26) = 10.15, p < 0.001. There was also a significant main effect of texture orientation relative to shading on perceived pigmentation, F(1, 13) = 22.38, p < 0.0005, but no interaction effect between texture and image orientation, F(2, 26) = 2.00, p = 0.16. For spherical surfaces with high-frequency texture, another repeated-measures ANOVA on the arc-sine transformed probability estimates again found a significant main effect of image orientation on perceived pigmentation, F(2, 26) = 11.54, p < 0.0005. Perceived pigmentation also showed a significant dependence on the relative orientation of the texture and the shading, F(1, 13) = 8.44, p < 0.05, which did not interact significantly with image orientation, F(2, 26) = 0.80, p = 0.46.
These results replicate the effects of texture orientation on perceived surface pigmentation found in the previous experiments. This finding was evident in the significant differences in perceived pigmentation between rotated and unrotated textures on planar and spherical surfaces. However, we also found that inverting images significantly reduced the salience of perceived texture. This was further verified by follow-up Bonferroni-corrected contrasts, which found a significant difference in perceived texture between upright images and inverted images (i.e., between image orientations at 0° and 180°) of bumpy spheres, t(13) = 2.96, p < 0.05, but not bumpy planes, t(13) = 0.99, p > 0.05. This suggests that the appearance of surface texture depends not only on low-level information about local shading gradients but also on constraints that embody illumination biases. All of the image structure was preserved across changes in image orientation. The decline in perceived pigmentation for inverted spheres is consistent with increased incompatibility between an illumination from above bias and shading flow consistent with inverted illumination. This is consistent with the view that violating the light from above assumption complicates the task of computing shading, which makes it more difficult to segment pigmentation from shading. The lack of decline in perceived pigmentation between upright and inverted planes may be explained by perceptual resolution of the ambiguity in local surface concavity/convexity by a perceived illumination from above.