Peripheral visual acuity is severely degraded compared to central vision (Low,
1951). There are both neural and optical reasons for this worsening of vision in the periphery. Neurally, the density of ganglion cells decreases (Curcio & Allen,
1990) whereas, optically, peripheral refractive errors differ from those of central vision (Ferree, Rand, & Hardy,
1931), and higher order aberrations are larger (Thibos, Cheney, & Walsh,
1987; Williams, Artal, Navarro, McMahon, & Brainard,
1996; Atchison & Scott,
2002; Lundström, Gustafsson, & Unsbo,
2009; Lundström, Mira-Agudelo, & Artal,
2009). Most studies argue that the peripheral field is specialized for motion perception, detection, low spatial frequencies, and low contrast and that peripheral resolution is insensitive to optical blur (cf. Brown,
1972; Millodot, Johnson, Lamont, & Leibowitz,
1975; Thibos, Walsh, & Cheney,
1987; Wood,
2002; Brooks, Tyrrell, & Franks,
2005; Schieber, Schlorholtz, & McCall,
2009; Atchison, Mathur, & Varnas,
2013). Despite poorer acuity, the quality of peripheral vision still has an impact on several important areas of vision research. First, peripheral vision is used extensively by patients with central visual field loss (Crossland, Culham, Kabanarou, & Rubin,
2005). Second, the optical quality on the peripheral retina might contribute to the enigmatic myopia development process (Smith,
2011). Third, a reduction in peripheral vision may be the first indication of serious diseases, such as glaucoma. Fourth, if the peripheral optical errors are corrected, detection acuity is improved as detection by aliasing becomes possible (Anderson,
1996; Wang, Thibos, & Bradley,
1997). Fifth, good peripheral vision is important for many daily tasks, such as locomotion, scene recognition, and driving (Wood,
2002; Lemmink, Dijkstra, & Visscher,
2005; Larson & Loschky,
2009).