One way attention can improve perceptual processing is by prioritizing the analysis of task-relevant over task-irrelevant sensory inputs (Broadbent,
1958). Accordingly, when more than one location or object is task relevant, attention may need to be divided to allow selection of noncontiguous stimuli. Several studies have examined whether observers can select sensory information independently from separate locations. Some suggested that attending multiple locations requires selection of one contiguous area of the visual field, encompassing all task-relevant stimuli (C. Eriksen & St. James,
1986; C. Eriksen & Yeh,
1985; W. Eriksen & Hoffman,
1973; Posner, Snyder, & Davidson,
1980), thus negating the possibility that spatial attention may be truly divided. Others concluded instead that dividing attention among noncontiguous locations is possible (Bichot, Cave, & Pashler,
1999; Castiello & Umiltà,
1992; Gobell, Tseng, & Sperling,
2004). This is particularly true when the display does not contain novel onset distractor stimuli, which automatically capture attention (Hahn & Kramer,
1998). Awh and Pashler (
2000) extended the generality of this result by demonstrating that observers can also suppress processing of novel onset distractors, at least partially, even when they occur between attended locations. Both neurophysiological and imaging studies, which indicated that neural activity and hemodynamic responses evoked by targets at separate locations can be simultaneously modulated by attention, even when signals evoked by distractors at intervening locations are not (McMains & Somers,
2004; Muller, Malinowski, Gruber, & Hillyard,
2003), have provided evidence consistent with the idea that attention can be split. Evidence from attentional tracking experiments, designed to examine the effects of target selection by minimizing those of processes, which may follow selection, such as target identification and memory (e.g., Duncan & Humphreys,
1989; Palmer,
1990), suggested initially that multiple targets can be selected in parallel (Pylyshyn & Storm,
1988). However, the generality of this conclusion was challenged by a number of observations. Attentional tracking can be improved by presenting targets in opposite visual hemifields, suggesting that tracking multiple objects in the same visual hemifield is not done in parallel (Alvarez & Cavanagh,
2005). Others found that when targets and distractors are closely spaced and moving unpredictably, increasing the number of targets from one to two leads to decrements in tracking performance. To account for these effects, it was suggested that attentional tracking is limited by how frequently samples of the targets' location can be obtained by the observers and since the overall sampling rate cannot be increased indefinitely, whenever the number of targets is increased, the sampling rate for each target must be diminished (d'Avossa, Shulman, Snyder, & Corbetta,
2006). A radical view has been put forth suggesting that perceptual processes have access to sensory information only through temporally discrete (VanRullen & Koch,
2003; VanRullen, Reddy, & Koch,
2005) and spatially limited “snapshots” (Dubois, Hamker, & VanRullen,
2009), so that in general two or more locations cannot be simultaneously attended. Accordingly, the ability to analyze simultaneous targets is limited by how rapidly attention can be moved among locations (Hogendoorn, Carlson, VanRullen, & Verstraten,
2010). Others have suggested that the ability to spatially divide attention is limited, but can be achieved by trading off the number of attended locations against the spatial resolution of the selection signal (Franconeri, Jonathan, & Scimeca,
2010).