Yet musicians' reported superiority is not limited to acoustic discriminations. The extent and cause of their advantage is still debated. Several studies reported that early musical training may enhance verbal skills, particularly verbal memory (e.g., Chan, Ho, & Cheung,
1998; Franklin et al.,
2008; Jakobson, Cuddy, & Kilgour,
2003). Other studies suggested that musical training boosts cognitive abilities in a broad manner (e.g., Bergman Nutley, Darki, & Klingberg,
2014; reviewed in Jacoby & Ahissar,
2013). Although enhancement of verbal span may be attributed to shared storage of verbal and nonverbal auditory stimuli, accounting for broader differences assumes the involvement of more central mechanisms. A prime candidate for musicians' broader underlying advantages is enhanced working memory. The term “working memory” refers to the mechanisms that underlie our ability to retain and manipulate information simultaneously (e.g., Baddeley,
1996). The efficiency of these mechanisms affects our planning and reasoning skills and also plays a role in perceptual and sensory-motor tasks, particularly when these require processing of sequentially presented stimuli (e.g., Broadway & Engle,
2011; Cho, Holyoak, & Cannon,
2007; Seidler, Bo, & Anguera,
2012; Süß, Oberauer, & Wittmann,
2002). Thus, an enhancement of central working memory processes is expected to upgrade performance in many tasks that depend on sequential processing. Several studies suggested that this is indeed the case with musicians. For example, an event-related potential study that assessed responses during auditory and visual oddball detection found that musicians' P300 response had a shorter latency. Because P300 is considered to indicate the timing of updating the content of working memory (i.e., “an oddball was just detected”), this observation was interpreted as implying that musicians' rate of updating is higher than that of nonmusicians (George & Coch,
2011).