Figure 6 presents a schematic illustration to account for the relation between perceptual grouping and contextual cueing in guiding attention. First, when considering a repeated “standard” display (i.e., without systematic grouping), it is assumed on the basis of a computational model by Brady and Chun (
2007) that contextual cueing relies primarily on the learning of local contextual associations between a target and a limited number of (about three) surrounding nontarget items (red lines in
Figure 6A; see also Olson & Chun,
2002). In order to encode these contextual associations, attention is not required—as evidenced, for instance, by the finding that an ignored subset of search items can nevertheless generate a reliable contextual-cueing effect (Jiang & Leung,
2005). This is reflected in our schematic model by a broad distribution of attention (gray attentional “spotlight” in
Figure 6) by default, such that contextual associations can be learned and used to guide search (see also Conci & von Mühlenen,
2011). However, the picture is different when the displays presented contain grouped regions, in which case contextual learning will primarily operate within the grouped, segmented region. As a result, reliable contextual associations are only, or primarily, formed when the target is located within this region (
Figure 6B,
C), but not for targets outside of the groupings (
Figure 6D,
E). Note that the preference of contextual learning to evolve within object boundaries is independent of grouping strength (which is depicted in
Figure 6 by the thickness of the gray dashed lines): Weak groupings (e.g., random squares;
Figure 6C,
E) impose comparable limits on contextual learning to strong groupings (e.g., squares;
Figure 6B,
D). Conversely, attention is only attracted by strong (e.g., square) groupings, whereas it remains distributed broadly when groupings are weak (e.g., random squares), illustrating that contextual learning is independent of the allocation of attention (Jiang & Leung,
2005), but dependent on the segmented groups. Thus, even subtle regularities in a given display that are too weak to attract attention may nevertheless interfere with contextual cueing (
Figure 6E). Segmented regions are therefore not necessarily equally effective for learning and attention: Contextual learning may already be modulated by relatively subtle grouping cues, whereas attentional selection only occurs for relatively salient configurations possessing a certain degree of “object-hood” (Kimchi et al.,
2007). In this view, salient groupings can directly influence the allocation of attention, while relatively nonsalient groupings exert only an indirect influence through the modulation of contextual learning.