While target locations are fixed for each old context in contextual cueing experiments, in everyday life, objects (e.g., a pan) are likely to change locations or to appear in several (recurring) places within their environments. Thus, ideally, statistical learning of contextual relationships should allow adaptation to changes and should include representations of multiple repeated target locations (Conci, Zellin, & Müller,
2012). However, several studies have reported that contextual cueing does not occur when targets are relocated within their otherwise invariant contexts (Chun & Jiang,
1998; Conci, Sun, & Müller,
2011; Makovski & Jiang,
2010; Manginelli & Pollmann,
2009). For example, in the study by Manginelli and Pollmann, observers learned to associate old contexts with fixed target locations. After this initial learning phase, targets were relocated to new, previously empty positions within their otherwise invariant contexts. Target relocation to previously empty positions was found to cancel the contextual-cueing effect, which failed to recover after repeated encounters with the new target locations. Reliable contextual cueing was not even observed when target relocations to previously empty positions were fairly permanent with at least twice as many presentations of the relocated targets relative to the initial target locations (Zellin, Conci, von Mühlenen, & Müller,
2013). This pattern of results suggests, on the one hand, that contextual cueing is essentially limited to
single-target learning (Zellin, Conci, von Mühlenen, & Müller,
2011); that is, each old-context display can be associated with only one repeated target location (and its immediate surround; see also Makovski & Jiang,
2010), meaning that visual search for other (new) repeated target locations will not be guided by the same old context. On the other hand, the observed lack of contextual cueing for relocated targets appearing at previously empty positions might be owing to the fact that, after having learned a particular context, observers did not expect targets to appear at previously empty positions (Jiang et al.,
2013; see Clark,
2013, for a theoretical approach to cognitive prediction models). In other words, the positions of relocated targets in the studies mentioned above were not predictable, which could have prevented their integration into the old contexts. Indeed, Conci et al. (
2011) reported successful contextual learning of two predictable target locations presented within one context (
Experiment 2). In each trial, search displays contained two targets at two different locations simultaneously (one was oriented left/right, one was pointing upward/downward). While both targets were present in each trial, observers only searched for one of the targets in one half of the experiment and for the other target in the other half. Reliable contextual cueing was observed for both target locations due to the continuous presence of the two targets, making both target locations predictable within their respective context (
multiple-target learning; see also Brady & Chun,
2007; Conci & Müller,
2012; Kunar & Wolfe,
2011). This finding suggests that predictability might be a key factor for successful adaptation to change in contextual learning.