In visual pop-out search, observers are required to detect a singleton feature target (e.g., the only red vs. green colored target, with color being the selection-relevant attribute) and report another target feature (e.g., target cut-off side left vs. right; cut-off “orientation” would be the response-defining attribute). Intertrial priming refers to the fact that reaction times (RTs) are faster for repeated relative to nonrepeated item attributes (see, e.g., Fecteau & Munoz,
2003; Found & Müller,
1996; Goolsby & Suzuki,
2001; Kristjánsson & Campana,
2010; Kristjánsson & Driver,
2008; Lamy, Antebi, Aviani, & Carmel,
2008; Maljkovic & Nakayama,
1994,
1996; Müller, Heller, & Ziegler,
1995; Olivers & Meeter,
2006). Such priming effects have been reported for several stimulus attributes, such as features (which includes target and distractor features; e.g., Lamy, Yashar, & Ruderman,
2010), dimensions (e.g., Found & Müller,
1996; Töllner, Rangelov, & Müller,
2012), and locations (e.g., Geyer et al.,
2010). Additionally, priming can also manifest for the search objects themselves (e.g., Huang, Holcombe, & Pashler,
2004; Yashar & Lamy,
2011). Of particular relevance in the present context is Huang et al.'s (
2004) report that priming from the target's selection-relevant feature (size) was contingent on repetitions versus changes of the target's response-relevant feature (orientation). This led to the proposal that priming effects in visual search reflect the operation of an episodic memory mechanism that stores information about selection-relevant, response-relevant, and irrelevant target features (see also Hillstrom,
2000). At the core of this account is the idea that when processing the current display, observers match the target to other recently experienced items, particularly the target on the immediately preceding trial. A full match or full nonmatch can facilitate processing, whereas a partial match can hinder search performance. Object-specific effects are usually considered as evidence for a “late” priming system, which influences processes that occur after the selection of the target by focal attention (see, e.g., Olivers & Meeter,
2006, for a discussion). The alternative view is that priming facilitates the selection of the target by focal attention (e.g., Kristjánsson & Nakayama,
2003; Müller et al.,
1995; Töllner, Gramann, Müller, Kiss, & Eimer,
2008). In recent years, a consensus has emerged that priming can aid processes of response selection and target selection, too (Lamy et al.,
2010; Töllner, Gramann, Müller, Kiss, & Eimer,
2008; Töllner et al.,
2012; Yashar & Lamy,
2011).