In this experiment, observers bisected an invisible line using a three-dimensional method of adjustment. Although no specific instructions regarding fixation were given, observers were encouraged to move their eyes across the vertex pair and to select the bisection point that appeared most satisfactory across these varying fixations. Under these conditions, the recorded bisection settings were stable between the two sessions for all four observers.
The back vertex pair consisted of two points displayed 14 cm apart in the frontal plane (symmetric with respect to the line of sight). In this vertex pair, two observers, JT and HB, exhibited rightward bisection biases, while the other two observers, IO and KB, did not show a bisection bias in the x direction. A leftward bisection bias, often observed in line bisection experiments in the frontal plane, did not occur for any of the observers with this vertex pair. However, in contrast to standard line bisection experiments in which observers are instructed to bisect a line using a one-dimensional judgment (along the line), in our experiment observers bisected the frontal vertex pair using a full three-dimensional adjustment. Being able to place the bisecting point anywhere in space, and not restricted to a position along the line, three observers chose settings significantly deviating from the bisecting line, either in depth (observers IO and JT), or in the vertical direction (observers JT and KB).
As mentioned in the “Introduction,” biases in perceived geometry that differ from Euclidean predictions have been attributed to an underlying Riemannian geometry of perceptual space with nonzero curvature (
Indow, 1991). Empirical studies in this vein, but typically carried out at greater distances from the observer than the present study, have found different values of the curvature parameter
k, both positive (indicating a spherical geometry) and negative (indicating a hyperbolic geometry). Curvature estimates have even been found to vary within individual observers if measured at different locations in space (
Koenderink et al., 2000) or when context objects are added to the scene (
Cuijpers et al., 2001;
Schoumans et al., 2000,
2002). In agreement with these previous results (which involved distances beyond grasp space), our results contradict a geometric interpretation of visual space, both qualitatively and quantitatively. Were the biases found in our study a result of a non-Euclidean geometry of constant curvature, we would expect all the bisection settings to be outside the triangle (for a positive curvature) or all inside (for a negative curvature). This is not true of most of our subjects, even in the small region of visual grasp space immediately in front of the observer. Second, given an empirically estimated value of curvature from a previous study, one can calculate the bias expected in the present experiment, which uses an inter-point distance that is far smaller. The predicted biases are much smaller than those we found. We conclude that our results cannot be attributed to an underlying Riemannian geometry of constant curvature (compare
Cuijpers, Kappers, & Koenderink, 2001,
2002;
Koenderink, van Doorn, & Lappin, 2000,
2003;
Todd, Oomes, Koenderink, & Kappers, 2001).