In the first experiment, we tested the sensitivity of the pursuit system during the open-loop period to target acceleration. In each block of trials, observers tracked targets that moved either left or right with a base speed chosen randomly from a set of five (4, 5, 6, 7, or 8 deg/s) and an acceleration chosen randomly from a set of six (0, 4, 8, 12, 16, or 30 deg/s
2). Each block of trials presented all possible stimulus combinations of base speeds and accelerations. Two blocks of trials (6 trials per stimulus per block) were run for each of the single spot and random-dot field conditions. (Subject SW did not perform this experiment with an acceleration of 30 deg/s
2.) To assess the pursuit response to target speed and acceleration, we sorted the same set of trials across either initial speed or acceleration respectively (see
Figure 1). This was done separately for data gathered using the spot and the RDC stimuli.
During the open-loop period, different patterns of responses were observed when the eye traces were sorted as a function of speed and as a function of acceleration.
Figure 2 shows representative average eye traces from one block of trials for one observer (rightward trials only). When sorted by speed, the traces appear to be better ordered and more clearly separated than when sorted by acceleration. As can be seen from the traces displayed in the first column, lower eye velocity and acceleration accompanied lower target speeds throughout most of the open loop period when the traces were sorted by speed (
Figure 2A and
2C). However, in the second column where the traces are displayed sorted by target acceleration (
Figure 2B and
2D), traces do not rank appropriately as a function of acceleration. It may seem unusual that
Figure 2A shows such small differences between the eye traces for the different speeds, but one must keep in mind that while each trace represents the average pursuit to targets that had a starting speed of 4, 6, or 8 deg/s, the targets within a speed accelerated at various rates. For example, for the trace labeled 4 deg/s, there were stimuli that accelerated at 0, 8, and 16 deg/s
2, so the average speed of the stimulus over the first 140 ms would have ranged from 4 to about 6 deg/s. Thus the average speed of the stimuli that the eye was attempting to follow was higher than the initial starting speed. In addition, we have previously shown that for single spots moving at speeds in the range used here, the difference in eye velocity was not that pronounced (
Heinen & Watamaniuk, 1998).
In
Figure 3, average eye acceleration as a function of target speed and target acceleration is plotted. The data show a tendency for all observers to generate higher eye acceleration with higher target accelerations (
Figure 3A), and higher target speeds (
Figure 3B). A similar pattern of results was obtained from the two observers tracking RDCs (
Figure 3C and
3D). While it appears that eye acceleration increases at a lower rate as a function of target acceleration than it does as a function of target speed, it could be that at the highest target accelerations, eye acceleration saturates. However,
Tychsen & Lisberger (1986) showed that human observers’ eye acceleration in the open-loop period reached levels in excess of 240 deg/s
2 for target speeds of 45 deg/s. Eye accelerations in our study reached only about 80 deg/s
2, far below the accelerations measured in the previous study. Therefore, the range of eye accelerations generated by our observers when they pursued accelerating stimuli appears not to be limited by saturation, rather by a reduced sensitivity of the pursuit system to retinal-image acceleration. We quantify the differences in these functions when we compare oculomotor to perceptual performance.