Version and vergence during a non-isovergent saccade are to a large extent independently controlled (
Collewijn, Erkelens, & Steinman, 1997). The saccade to a visual target includes not only a preprogrammed change in version (
Robinson, 1964), but also a preprogrammed change in vergence (
Semmlow, Hung, Horng, & Ciuffreda, 1993). Suppose that the visual system knows, with good precision, the magnitude of the vergence component in a saccade. How might it use this information to measure the relative disparity between two targets?
A paradox is that in order to plan the vergence component of a saccade in the first place, the absolute
retinal disparity of the new target must be estimated. Sheliga and Miles (
2003) report in this issue that perceived depth contributes to such an estimate, and of course the retinal disparity of the new target can also be measured from the retinal images. If the estimate is sufficiently accurate and precise, however, then the visual system already knows the relative disparity between the two targets; in this case, there would be no need to monitor vergence change during the change of fixation from one target to the other. However, the system may know actual changes in vergence eye posture better than it knows the change required to fixate a new target. First, there is some evidence that vergence eye movements are characterized by an initial, preprogrammed component that achieves much but not all of the required vergence change, followed by a second, slower, feedback-controlled component that closes the remaining fixation disparity (
Semmlow, Hung, Horng, & Ciuffreda, 1994). In essence, the system can be described as habitually relying on the feedback-controlled phase to improve on its inaccurate initial guess. Second, the fact that perceived depth is more accurate when eye movements are allowed is itself evidence that measurements of actual vergence change are of higher quality than the retinal measurements of disparity that would be needed to plan accurate saccades: observers are better able to null the depth interval between widely separated targets when they are allowed to make eye movements (
Enright, 1991a,
1991b), and perceived depth for large disparity intervals is poor when eye movements between near and far targets are disallowed (
Foley & Richards, 1972). For large intertarget separations and intertarget disparities, the visual system appears not to rely on retinal disparity alone to generate perceived depth, so it is likely that retinally measured disparity cannot support the planning of accurate vergence eye movements either.