Consistent evidence has recently accumulated showing that visual attention can alter perception. Attention increases perceived contrast (Carrasco, Ling, & Read,
2004), spatial frequency or gap size (Gobell & Carrasco,
2005), and color saturation (Fuller & Carrasco,
2006). In addition to changing static stimulus features, attention has been shown to alter the perception of dynamic stimulus properties, like perceived speed of motion (Turatto, Vescovi, & Valsecchi,
2007) and motion coherence (Liu, Fuller, & Carrasco,
2006). Exogenous or bottom-up shifts of attention take place when salient events, such as, for example, abrupt visual onsets, occur in the visual field (Jonides,
1981; Nakayama & Mackeben,
1989). On the contrary, endogenous or top-down control is achieved when attention is allocated according to the observers' goals and intentions (e.g., Posner,
1980). To date, evidence that attention can alter perception comes from studies using paradigms that manipulated exogenous attention. Within the class of top-down mechanisms that are possibly involved in attentional control an important one is visual working memory (WM). WM refers to functional storage mechanism(s) that are devoted to temporarily maintaining information available to cognitive systems for further processing (Baddeley,
1986).