Free
Research Article  |   May 2008
Perceptual multistability predicted by search model for Bayesian decisions
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision May 2008, Vol.8, 12. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/8.5.12
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Rashmi Sundareswara, Paul R. Schrater; Perceptual multistability predicted by search model for Bayesian decisions. Journal of Vision 2008;8(5):12. https://doi.org/10.1167/8.5.12.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Perceptual multistability refers to the phenomenon of spontaneous perceptual switching between two or more likely interpretations of an image. Although frequently explained by processes of adaptation or hysteresis, we show that perceptual switching can arise as a natural byproduct of perceptual decision making based on probabilistic (Bayesian) inference, which interprets images by combining probabilistic models of image formation with knowledge of scene regularities. Empirically, we investigated the effect of introducing scene regularities on Necker cube bistability by flanking the Necker cube with fields of unambiguous cubes that are oriented to coincide with one of the Necker cube percepts. We show that background cubes increase the time spent in percepts most similar to the background. To characterize changes in the temporal dynamics of the perceptual alternations beyond percept durations, we introduce Markov Renewal Processes (MRPs). MRPs provide a general mathematical framework for describing probabilistic switching behavior in finite state processes. Additionally, we introduce a simple theoretical model consistent with Bayesian models of vision that involves searching for good interpretations of an image by sampling a posterior distribution coupled with a decay process that favors recent to old interpretations. The model has the same quantitative characteristics as our human data and variation in model parameters can capture between-subject variation. Because the model produces the same kind of stochastic process found in human perceptual behavior, we conclude that multistability may represent an unavoidable by-product of normal perceptual (Bayesian) decision making with ambiguous images.

Introduction
The human visual system is remarkably good at producing crisp and consistent perceptions of the world. Underlying these crisp and consistent percepts are sophisticated methods for resolving sensory ambiguity and conflicts, many of which have been successfully described using Bayesian inference (Kersten, Mamassian, & Yuille, 2004). However, we experience a breakdown of this consistency when viewing stimuli that produce phenomena like perceptual multistability and binocular rivalry. The question motivating this study is to what extent perceptual multistability can be viewed as resulting from the process of perceptual decision making based on Bayesian inference, without recourse to special mechanisms or processing. 
Many theories have emphasized the role that non-inferential explanations like neural adaptation may play in generating multistability (Attneave, 1971; Blake, 1989; Lehky, 1988; Long, Toppino, & Mondin, 1992; Taylor & Aldridge, 1974). However, recent evidence points to both bottom-up and top-down influences on multistability (Leopold & Logothetis, 1999). Top-down influences include attention (e.g., Pelton & Solley, 1968; Peterson & Gibson, 1994; Rock, Hall, & Davis, 1994; van Ee, Noest, Brascamp, & van den Berg, 2006), intention (Peterson, Harvey, & Weidenbacher, 1991; Toppino, 2003), recognition (Peterson & Gibson, 1994), semantic content (Davis, Schiffman, & Greist-Bousquet, 1990; Walker, 1978), and voluntary control (Hol, Koene, & van Ee, 2003; Meng & Tong, 2004; Toppino, 2003; van Ee, van Dam, & Brouwer, 2005; van Ee et al., 2006). These many and diverse influences suggest multistable perception may be better viewed as resulting from a general process of perceptual (Bayesian) decision making than a specific low-level mechanism. In particular, it has been suggested that multistability may be an extreme case of ambiguity in normal perceptual processing (van Ee, Adams, & Mamassian, 2003) where several different percepts are equally probable. 
If multistability results from an extreme case of ambiguity in normal perceptual processing, then manipulations that reduce ambiguity in normal vision should increase perceptual stability when viewing multistable stimuli. One way to reduce ambiguity involves placing an ambiguous image in a global context that promotes a perceptual interpretation. Previous studies have shown that moving multistable stimuli can be stabilized by the motion of background elements (Dawson, 1987; Kramer & Yantis, 1997). Similar manipulations for stationary rivalrous stimuli increase dominance times (Alais & Blake, 1999; Blake & Logothetis, 2002; Sobel & Blake, 2002). 
In this paper, we investigate the effects of background (context) on the dynamics of perceptual bistability` while viewing the Necker cube. From an ecological or Bayesian perspective, background context can provide information about the frequency of occurrence of attributes in an image. Because the ambiguity in the Necker cube can be thought of as viewpoint ambiguity-the dominant percepts are consistent with two viewpoints of a single cube (Mamassian & Landy, 1998), we introduced background context that carried viewpoint information. In particular, our context stimuli consist of fields of unambiguously oriented cubes whose orientations are consistent with one of the dominant Necker cube interpretations. These background objects flank a central Necker cube (without overlap). Similar to previous effects of context on rivalrous stimuli (Sobel & Blake, 2002), we find context lengthens the durations of the supported percepts. 
To better quantify the effects of context on the dynamics of multistability, we developed a new approach to the analysis of empirical data collected for this phenomenon. We developed a general non-parametric descriptive framework for analyzing multistability data based on Markov Renewal Processes. The approach describes perceptual switching in terms of a discrete state variable which undergoes probabilistic transitions after random times in a state. The approach overcomes several limitations of previous analysis methods. It can capture the temporal dynamics of the switching process, contingencies between states, and durations and can provide tools to relate different measures of perceptual behavior. We provide a brief exposition of the key ideas and mathematical properties of the approach in the supplementary material, “Markov Renewal Processes for Bistability.” 
We use our analysis to reveal the dynamics of multistability in the presence of context. We found non-trivial dynamics that were modulated by context, with strong biases toward one perceptual interpretation on initial viewing that partially dissipate in subsequent viewing, similar to those reported by Mamassian and Goutcher (2005) for rivalrous stimuli. In addition, we show how the analysis approach can be used express relationships between measures, predict covariaton between the means and the variabilities of percept durations and to test stationarity (the property of stationarity measures whether or not the probability distributions governing perceptual state transitions vary across time). 
Lastly, we show how multistability could result from the normal dynamics of perceptual Bayesian inference and decision making. Similar to van Ee et al. (2003), we assume that multistability arises when perceptual inference produces ambiguity that can be described by a multi-modal posterior distribution on the parameters of interest (e.g., surface slant, viewpoint, etc). The novelty of our approach was to provide a simple but specific method of implementing a perceptual decision that can account for the origin of the temporal dynamics in multistability. In particular, we show that searching for good interpretations by sampling a multimodal posterior, together with memory decay which values recent samples in time more than older samples, results in multistable behavior similar to that exhibited by participants. We believe that treating multistability within the context of a general approach to perception (Bayesian inference) has the potential to reconcile many of the diverse results observed for multistable perception. 
Methods
Task
Participants continuously viewed a Necker cube for 10-s intervals while fixating at a point in the center of the cube ( Figure 1). Perceptual states were elicited via button press at semi-random intervals by the sound of a beep. A trial was defined as a single event of an arrow press at the sound of a beep to indicate the orientation of the current perception of the Necker cube (see Figure 1). We refer to the two dominant interpretations in terms of user's viewpoint with respect to the Necker cube—“ V A” indicates the percept was consistent with the viewpoint of the subject being above the cube in 3D space (resulting in the top surface perceived as in front), and “ V B” indicates that the percept was consistent with a viewpoint below the cube and hence viewing the bottom surface. At the sound of a beep, if the user was currently experiencing the V A percept, they were instructed to press the “up” arrow key, and if they were experiencing the V B percept, they were to press the “down” arrow key. If any percept other than V A or V B appeared, they were instructed not to press any key. 
Figure 1
 
Necker stimulus used in the experiment. Participants fixated the central green dot and reported their perceptual state via key-press when cued by the sound of a beep.
Figure 1
 
Necker stimulus used in the experiment. Participants fixated the central green dot and reported their perceptual state via key-press when cued by the sound of a beep.
Stimuli and apparatus
The Necker cube was rendered in OpenGL and oriented at 45 degrees away from the x-axis and 55 degrees away from the y-axis such that vertically and horizontally it subtended a visual angle of 3.16 degrees. Participants were directed to maintain fixation on a tiny green dot rendered in the center of the cube. The user's chin rested on a chin rest which was 71 cm away from the center of the 24.2-cm-high monitor. 
Training
All participants ran in the following training sessions before the main experimental data was collected. A training session consisted of the following (in the order given): 
  1.  
    40 repetitions of V A/ V B key association practice where a single unambiguous cube would be presented in the middle of the screen, and the subject was asked to indicate with a key press what percept they were experiencing.
  2.  
    40 repetitions of a task designed to help participants discriminate between orientations around the x-axis. We refer to this as the “orientation discrimination” task. Two columns of 100 unambiguously oriented cubes appeared as shown Figures 2 and 3. The two columns differed in orientation from the Necker cube in the x-axis by random offsets with opposite (randomly-chosen) sign (e.g., +6 and −3.5 degrees). The offsets were sampled from a Gaussian distribution with a mean of 7 degrees and a standard deviation of 1. The user was asked to indicate which column appeared more fronto-parallel.
  3.  
    The Necker cube was presented for 1 minute. The user was asked to passively focus on the green dot in the middle of the screen. No beeps were sounded and no key presses were required.
  4.  
    The Necker cube was presented in the middle of the screen for 20 s. Beeps were generated using a Gaussian distribution with a mean of 0.5 s and a standard deviation of 100 milliseconds after registering the previous response. The subject was asked to quickly respond with a key press at the sound of the beep, which resulted in an average inter-response interval of 1.2 s. Five 20-s blocks were collected from each subject, which constituted the base-rate data for the no-context condition.
Figure 2
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V A orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
Figure 2
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V A orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
Figure 3
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V B orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
Figure 3
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V B orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
No time limits were enforced in the training session. The session was usually completed in 6–7 minutes. 
Context experiment
The experiment consisted of 100 blocks where each block consisted of 10 trials. During the duration of a block, the Necker cube appeared in the middle of the screen and was flanked by two columns of 100 unambiguously oriented cubes on either side of the cube as shown in Figures 4 and 5, in the exact conditions of the orientation discrimination task described in the training session. The orientations of the cubes in the two columns were either both V A or both V B. The orientation of the cubes for a block was randomly permuted such that at the end of 100 blocks, 50 of them were V A oriented and 50 of them were V B oriented. In a block, the beeps were generated from a Gaussian distribution with a mean of 0.5 s and a standard deviation of 100 milliseconds (similar to the training session). It took an average of 0.5 s for the user to respond to a beep (by pressing an arrow key). Therefore, the duration of a block lasts for 10–11 s as 10 events of arrow presses are recorded for every block. If the subject took longer than 1.2 s to respond to the beep, a double beep was sounded and the block restarts. A 2- to 3-minute break was provided at the end of 20 blocks. 
Figure 4
 
An instance of the V A context stimulus.
Figure 4
 
An instance of the V A context stimulus.
Figure 5
 
An instance of V B context stimulus.
Figure 5
 
An instance of V B context stimulus.
Participants
Twenty-one participants ran in three 1-hour experimental sessions scheduled over 3 days. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants were either financially compensated or given extra credits points in an introductory Psychology class. 
Analysis
Data from multi-stability experiments are composed of sequences of durations spent in alternating states. A key characteristic of this data is switching between discrete perceptual states with stochastic phase durations between switch events. Estimating the distribution of phase durations is an essential part of analyzing bistability data. However, it is not sufficient to simply model phase durations with histograms. To do so would ignore the possibilities that phase duration distributions may depend on previous perceptual states and may change across time. Our goal was to develop analysis methods that could more completely capture the temporal dynamics of perceptual switching behavior. 
As pointed out by Mamassian and Goutcher (2005), to capture the temporal dynamics of perceptual switching, it is not sufficient to measure only response probabilities or phase durations because these measures are insensitive to temporal changes in the distribution of phase durations. To rectify this, Mamassian and Goutcher introduced three additional measures—transient preference, reversal probabilities and survival probabilities to capture the time-varying dynamics of perceptual switching. We also measure survival probabilities but use a non-parametric estimation method (Mamassian and Goutcher fit cumulative Gaussian functions). In addition, we measure cumulative transition distribution functions (these are related to the inverse of phase duration) separately for each state. Both these measures are conditioned on particular times (rather than averaged across all occurrences in the sequence) and are conditioned on perceptual states as described below. Ultimately, these measures were chosen because of their simple relationships to a general mathematical model of probabilistic switching processes called Markov Renewal Processes, as described below in this section and in the Supplementary data, “Markov Renewal Processes for Bistability.” First we describe the measures and subsequently explain how they were estimated from the data. 
Preliminaries
Our data consists of sequences of response/time pairs. For the kth block of responses, let D k = {( r 1, t 1), ( r 2, t 2), …, ( r N, t N)}, where r j denotes responses, encoded as 0 for V A and 1 for V B, and t j represents a recorded response time measured from the start of the response period. We also use superscripts to indicate the block number of a measurement (e.g., t j k represents the jth measurement from the kth block). All of our measures were conditioned on starting times, defined for each sequence as:  
τ 0 k = min j { t j k | r j k = 0 } τ 1 k = min j { t j k | r j k = 1 } .
(1)
In words, a starting time is the first time a state is observed within a response block. 
In general, all measures were computed using the following steps. Responses in each block were classified according to the event type of interest (e.g., whether a transition has occurred). Interpolation is used to construct a time-continuous indicator function from the classified events separately for each block. Estimates of the desired probability functions are generated from averages of indicator functions across blocks. 
Estimating survival and response probabilities
Survival probabilities are defined as the probability of observing the same state i after an elapsed time t:  
P ( s ( τ + t ) = i | s ( τ ) = i ) .
(2)
Note that survival probabilities do not encode the time spent continuously in a state. In the elapsed time t, there may have been any number of state transitions away from i, as long as the state returns i to by time t
Estimating survival probabilities is complicated by the unavailability of continuous time measurements of perceptual states. To estimate survival probabilities at each time t, we interpret the response/time sequences as discrete samples from the continuous survival probability function. We classify response events for each block based on whether the initial state was V A or V B and linearly interpolate between response times. Let S k( t) represent the interpolated indicator function for the kth block. For each t falling between two response times, S k( t) is given by (let the subscripts 0 and 1 in S k( t) represent states V A and V B, respectively):  
S 0 k ( t ) = ( 1 r j k ) ( t ( t j k τ 0 k ) t j + 1 k t j k ) + ( 1 r j + 1 k ) ( ( t j + 1 k τ 0 k ) t t j + 1 k t j k ) f o r ( t j k τ 0 k ) t ( t j + 1 k τ 0 k ) ,
(3)
if the initial response is of type V A. The interpolated indicator function for initial response of type V B is similar. 
No extrapolation is performed for times t greater than any of the response times. Instead, times exceeding the last response are treated as missing data for that block. To keep track of which times points are valid after τ 0 k, we compute a second indicator variable that marks valid time points:  
J 0 k ( t ) = { 1 0 < t < max ( t i k τ 0 k ) 0 o t h e r w i s e
(4)
The survival probability estimate ( P S) for state i is computed as:  
P ^ S ( s ( τ + t ) = i | s ( τ ) = i ) = k = 1 # b l o c k s S i k ( t ) k = 1 # b l o c k s J i k ( t ) .
(5)
Response probabilities ( P R) are similar but simpler to estimate. The recorded response sequences for each block were interpolated and averaged incorporating only valid times as above. 
Estimating cumulative transition probabilities
We also measured the cumulative transition probability from each state, conditioned on initial states with times τ 0 k and τ 1 k (where subscripts 0 and 1 represent states V A and V B). Note that the time spent in a state is the same as the time observed before a transition. To construct an indicator function for a transition from a state, V A or V B, we classify the responses subsequent to first response in each block as preceding or following the first observed state transition. Beginning with the initial state, subsequent responses are classified as 0 until the first state-change response is observed. The remaining responses in the block are classified as 1. The classification produces an indicator variable Z k( t). Let Z 0 k denote the transition sequence described above for state V A. Linear interpolation is used to construct indicator functions for cumulative probability until transition. For the cumulative transition probability of state V A, the indicator function T 0 k( t) is given by:  
T 0 k ( t ) = Z 0 k ( t ( t j k τ 0 k ) t j + 1 k t j k ) + Z 0 k ( ( t j + 1 k τ 0 k ) t t j + 1 k t j k ) f o r ( t j k τ 0 k ) t ( t j + 1 k τ 0 k ) .
(6)
We computed indicator functions for valid times as before and generated estimates of cumulative transition probabilities ( P T) by averaging indicator functions across blocks:  
P ^ T ( N i ( t ) > 0 | s ( τ ) = i ) = k = 1 # b l o c k s T i k ( t ) k = 1 # b l o c k s J i k ( t ) , ( 7 a )
(7a)
where N i( t) is the number of transitions from state i up to time t after the start time. Note that there is an event equivalence between this event and the time D i, continuously spent in a state. Thus, another way of expressing Equation 7a is:  
P ^ T ( D i < t | s ( τ ) = i ) = k = 1 # b l o c k s T i k ( t ) k = 1 # b l o c k s J i k ( t ) .
(7b)
In Equation 7b, we are expressing the idea that P T computes the cumulative probability that the duration of a percept is less than t. Phase duration means and variances were computed directly from numerical estimates of the moments of the cumulative transition probability curves. 
Errors in estimates
Errors in probability function estimates were assessed by resampling data across blocks using standard bootstrap procedures (Efron & Tibshirani, 1993). In particular, blocks were re-sampled with replacement and 50 bootstrap estimates of the survival, response and cumulative transition probability functions were generated using the 0.632+ procedure (Efron & Tibshirani, 1997). Standard errors of the probability estimates were computed via standard deviations of the set of bootstrap probability function estimates. 
We tested several assumptions of our estimation method by assessing whether modified estimates would fall outside of the 95% bootstrap confidence intervals. In particular, we found that changing the interpolation method to cubic interpolation had no significant effect on estimates nor did assigning transition times to the midpoint between response times. We also assessed whether selecting only those blocks where τ 0 k and τ 1 k occurred in the first 1–3 s would change the estimates but also found no significant effect. 
Note that the estimates are noisier for the elapsed times approaching 10 s after the first recorded percept time; this is because there were fewer blocks in which the appropriate initial state occurred at the end of the block. 
General framework for analyzing multi-stability data
Markov Renewal Processes (MRPs) (Ross, 1992) are general, well-characterized stochastic processes that model transitions among discrete states. In particular, MRPs make transitions between discrete states via a Markov chain and sample the amount of time spent in each state from a set of state-contingent temporal distribution functions. 
MRPs provide greater clarity on the analysis of multistability behavior by expressing relationships between different measures and address challenging issues in data interpretation such as the temporal dynamics and state stationarity. 
Some of the key properties useful for quantifying bistable data include the following: temporal contingencies can be measured by conditioning on past events (we focus on first order, or just the previous event); non-stationarity can be diagnosed; MRPs allow for arbitrary temporal distribution functions for perceptual durations; and many of the key predictions and relationships involve moments rather than full distribution functions (so that distributional assumptions can be avoided). Finally, in previous work (Schrater & Sundareswara, 2006), we showed how MRPs can be generated by the kind of theoretical model described in section “Theoretical framework for interpretation” below. We describe the properties of MRPs more fully in the supplementary material, “Markov Renewal Processes for Bistability.” 
Results
Average effects of context
The average effects of context on Necker cube perception are reported in Figures 6, 7, and 8. We analyze response data compiled across 20 participants (one participant was excluded from the analysis as a clear outlier). If context partially disambiguates perception of the Necker cube, we would expect background contexts that suggest view from above to increase V A percept durations but may also decrease V B percept durations. We would also expect to see an initial bias toward V A first in the first few responses. 
Figure 6
 
P R’ measure: Response of V B percept as a function of time. The blue curve represents baseline data (no context), the red curve data are P R when the context shown was V A, and green represents P Rwhen the context was V B context. The figure shows large initial response biases both with and without context. View from above context can be described as producing the same initial bias as baseline, but preserves more of the initial bias to asymptote around 0.3. V B context produces a strong bias toward V B responses that decreases to asymptote around 0.6 after 5 s. Dashed lines represented ±1 bootstrap SE of the estimate.
Figure 6
 
P R’ measure: Response of V B percept as a function of time. The blue curve represents baseline data (no context), the red curve data are P R when the context shown was V A, and green represents P Rwhen the context was V B context. The figure shows large initial response biases both with and without context. View from above context can be described as producing the same initial bias as baseline, but preserves more of the initial bias to asymptote around 0.3. V B context produces a strong bias toward V B responses that decreases to asymptote around 0.6 after 5 s. Dashed lines represented ±1 bootstrap SE of the estimate.
Figure 7
 
Average effect of context on cumulative transition probability conditioned on the initial perceptual state. The x-axis indicates time during a block and the y-axis is probability of having switched within the time indicated on the axis. Shows P T curves, given the initial percept is (A) view from above, V A, and (B) view from below, V B. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on cumulative transition probability (red = V A and green = V B, dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error) compared to no context (blue curves). Durations (time before transition) spent in the V A initial state (Figure A) are prolonged by V A context and shortened by V B context. However, while initial V B durations (Figure B) are prolonged by consistent context, there is little suppression of V B percept from view-from-above context when compared to the no-context (base-rate) condition. Note that context effects on distribution functions involve simultaneous shape and scale changes, corresponding to correlated changes in the phase duration means and variances.
Figure 7
 
Average effect of context on cumulative transition probability conditioned on the initial perceptual state. The x-axis indicates time during a block and the y-axis is probability of having switched within the time indicated on the axis. Shows P T curves, given the initial percept is (A) view from above, V A, and (B) view from below, V B. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on cumulative transition probability (red = V A and green = V B, dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error) compared to no context (blue curves). Durations (time before transition) spent in the V A initial state (Figure A) are prolonged by V A context and shortened by V B context. However, while initial V B durations (Figure B) are prolonged by consistent context, there is little suppression of V B percept from view-from-above context when compared to the no-context (base-rate) condition. Note that context effects on distribution functions involve simultaneous shape and scale changes, corresponding to correlated changes in the phase duration means and variances.
Figure 8
 
Effects of context on survival probabilities ( P S). (A) survival of V A initial percept shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. (B) survival of V B initial percepts shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on survival probability functions (red = V A and green = V B) compared to no context (blue curves). Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error. Note that all survival curves have the same asymptote as the response probability functions (which they should if MRPs provide a sufficient data model) shown in Figure 6.
Figure 8
 
Effects of context on survival probabilities ( P S). (A) survival of V A initial percept shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. (B) survival of V B initial percepts shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on survival probability functions (red = V A and green = V B) compared to no context (blue curves). Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error. Note that all survival curves have the same asymptote as the response probability functions (which they should if MRPs provide a sufficient data model) shown in Figure 6.
Figure 6 shows response probability ( P R) as a function of time for view from below responses V B (i.e., the percentage of time participants will respond V B at the sound of the beep). The base-rate/no-context data (blue curve) show that participants are biased toward V A, percepts (viewpoint from above) quite strongly for initial responses made early in a response block and asymptote at approximately 40% after 5–6 s. The fact that the asymptotic value is not 50% shows that percepts were biased toward viewpoint from above on average without context. The red curve ( V A context) shows increased V A response to V A context. The green curve shows increased V Bresponses to V B context. However, while V B context strongly biased initial responses toward view from below, V A context stimuli do not significantly change initial bias over baseline. Overall, V B context produces a larger change in response behavior than V A context. V A context may produce a smaller shift because of the bias toward V A responses in the no-context data. 
While response probabilities showed overall effects of context, they do not capture state-contingent effects—for example, it does not answer whether context has differential effects on transition probabilities of similar and dissimilar percepts. We use two related measures to capture state-contingent effects, cumulative transition probability ( P T) and survival probability curves ( P S), where both are conditioned on the initial perceptual state. Based on response probability results that show increased responses to percepts that match the context, we would expect context to increase time until transition and increase survival probabilities for context-consistent perceptual states. 
Cumulative transition probability (cumulative probability that a switch has occurred by time “ t”) ( P T) and survival probability ( P S) data are shown in Figures 7 and 8. In both figures, panel a shows results for initial V A percepts panel b shows results for initial V B percepts. Clear effects of context for both types of percept are observed. When context is consistent with the initial percept, this initial percept takes longer to transition (i.e., consistent percepts are less likely to transition with consistent context) and survival probabilities increase. In addition, inconsistent context has the effect of suppressing the unsupported percept, increasing the likelihood of transition. However, suppressive effects are only large for view-from-below initial perceptual states V A. This might be related to biases toward V A perceptual states found in the baseline conditions. For example, because V A percepts are more frequent in baseline viewing (in terms of durations and initial frequencies), there may be a ceiling effect for suppression. The relationship between the two measures is shown in the Supplementary data, “Markov Renewal Process for Bistability.” 
Next, we examined how the mean phase duration of both percepts was affected by context. Literature in binocular rivalry (Blake & Logothetis, 2002; Sobel & Blake, 2002) suggests that in the presence of a supporting context, durations of the matched percept are prolonged, but durations of the incongruent percept are not affected (Levelt's second proposition), suggesting that perhaps there are different neural events associated with increasing and decreasing perceptual durations. 
However, in Figure 9 we show that not only were supported percept times lengthened but inconsistent percept times were shortened as well—although the amount of shortening in duration is much smaller. Thus, we see no need to invoke different neural processes to explain our lengthened and shortened times in our bistability data. 
Figure 9
 
Effect of consistent and inconsistent context on mean percept durations. Mean percept durations with context are plotted against mean durations in the baseline condition, for each combination of percept and context type. Error bars represent 99% bootstrap confidence intervals. The blue line represents the values predicted if context had no effect. Points above the line correspond to percept durations increased by context (prolonging), while points below the line indicate context-induced shortening.
Figure 9
 
Effect of consistent and inconsistent context on mean percept durations. Mean percept durations with context are plotted against mean durations in the baseline condition, for each combination of percept and context type. Error bars represent 99% bootstrap confidence intervals. The blue line represents the values predicted if context had no effect. Points above the line correspond to percept durations increased by context (prolonging), while points below the line indicate context-induced shortening.
Theoretical framework for interpretation
Probabilistic approach to Bayesian decision making via sampling
The function of the brain's visual system is to make informed guesses or inferences about the world given varying levels of uncertain retinal input. Inferences resulting from Bayesian statistics have been widely employed to describe visual processing from the level of neuronal behavior (Pouget, Dayan, & Zemel, 2003) to high-level visual processing (Yuille & Kersten, 2006). The purpose of this section is to show how bistable switching behavior might also arise from probabilistic models of perceptual inference and decision making. By providing a simple method for generating spontaneous switching in a Bayesian framework, we offer an alternative to the idea that explanations of perceptual bistability require specialized neural mechanisms whose job is to implement switching. At minimum, theories of bistability should provide explanations of (1) the existence of multiple interpretations, (2) the awareness of only one interpretation at a time, and (3) spontaneous switching between percepts. 
The first two criteria are natural byproducts of using Bayesian decision theory to construct computational models of perception (Kersten & Schrater, 2002). Models of perception formulated using Bayesian decision theory treat percepts as decisions about scene (world) properties based on a combination of prior visual information (priors) and knowledge about image formulation and scene/image regularities (likelihood). Decisions have two parts—an inferential process that computes probabilities across interpretations and a selection process that chooses high-probability interpretations (and may incorporate costs; Kersten & Schrater, 2002). 
In Bayesian decision theory, the presence of a multimodal posterior distribution function signals ambiguity in perception. For example, part of perceptual inference for the Necker cube figure is to decide the viewpoint direction θ from which the image I of the cube was taken (we do not address the other part, which involves an inference about the shape of the wireframe object). Given that the object's shape is inferred as cuboidal, the posterior probability P( θI) will be bimodal (see Figure 10), with equal peaks given a uniform prior on viewpoint. Bayesian inference can also incorporate viewpoint information (gathered from experience or background context) to bias perception toward more probable views of the cube. Previous work by Mamassian and Landy (1998) shows that human observers are biased toward a viewpoint from above (our “no context” condition data suggests this as well). In Bayesian models, percepts result from selecting a good interpretation, typically by choosing the viewpoint with maximal posterior probability. When the maximum is not unique, Bayesian decision theory provides no mechanism for choosing between equally good interpretations. Thus, while criteria 1 and 2 are natural byproducts of using Bayesian decision theory, criterion 3 (the spontaneous switching of percepts) is not. 
Figure 10
 
An illustration of the probability densities used in the model. The interpretation ambiguity is represented by a bi-modal density function on cube orientation θ (blue curve). The prior P(θ) encodes biases toward one of the interpretations, including the effects of context (green curve). The product of the likelihood and the prior produces the (normalized) posterior probability (red curve), from which we sample in our simulations.
Figure 10
 
An illustration of the probability densities used in the model. The interpretation ambiguity is represented by a bi-modal density function on cube orientation θ (blue curve). The prior P(θ) encodes biases toward one of the interpretations, including the effects of context (green curve). The product of the likelihood and the prior produces the (normalized) posterior probability (red curve), from which we sample in our simulations.
However, we found that spontaneous switching can arise when Bayesian decisions are implemented by a process that generates a set of samples from the posterior distribution across time and selects a percept from the largest probability sample in the set. We describe this process in detail below. The key idea of the approach is to implement decision making by a sampling procedure, in accord with some recent theories of how Bayesian decision theory may be neurally implemented. In particular, Lee and Mumford (2003) and Pouget et al. (2003), point out that probabilistic population coding models of cortex can be thought of as representing probability distributions in terms of samples from the distribution. In this approach, receptive fields of neurons in the population encode regions of the interpretation parameter space, and firing rates encode the probability of a sample. Whether or not this interpretation of neural coding is an accurate reflection of biology, sampling constitutes a fundamental method for implementing Bayesian decisions, and the brain's computations may perform something equivalent. In the next section, we explain how switching can spontaneously arise in Bayesian decisions based on sampling. 
Bistability from a sampling model for perceptual decision making
We assume the brain performs Bayesian inference on the Necker cube figure and computes samples from the (bimodal) posterior distribution, representing the two dominant viewpoint interpretations. A decision process operates on this posterior and brings the best interpretation into awareness and memory. More precisely, we make the following assumptions: 
  1.  
    Posterior probabilities are represented by a set of samples, updated across time. In particular, at a sequence of times t i (on a neural scale), the brain explores the posterior distribution by collecting a new set of samples and discarding the oldest samples. Samples, consist of both the parameter values θ( t i) and the associated posterior probabilities, w( t i), i.e., {( θ( t 1), w( t 1)),…, ( θ( t N), w( t N))}, where w( t 1) = P( θ( t 1)∣ I) are weights that represent the posterior probabilities when first sampled and t i are consecutive time points. Weights represent the quality of the interpretation associated with a sample, and they are discounted by a memory decay process.
  2.  
    Memory decay expresses the idea that the quality of an old interpretation decreases with time. We assume simple exponential discounting of a memory sample's weight by the age of the sample.
  3.  
    Perceptual decisions result from choosing the sample with the highest discounted weight. This sample's parameter value is chosen as the current interpretation and brought into memory. The memory sample's discounted posterior probability w( t i) represents the quality of the interpretation; this is also stored in memory. The interpretation process is illustrated in Figure 11. These basic assumptions produce spontaneous perceptual switching similar to human behavior.
Figure 11
 
An illustration of the sampling procedure. (A) A bimodal posterior over viewpoint parameter ( x-axis). The two modes correspond to the two interpretations: V A percept and V B percept. (B) The probability values for samples chosen earlier in panel a shown: red = V A percept, green = V B percept. (C) 5 samples in window are shown. Each sample is generated at the point in time as indicated by time ( x-axis) axis. The red corresponds to the V A percept and the green corresponds to the V B percept. The exponential decay factor is applied to the samples (i.e., each sample's probability is weighted by the exponential decay factor's value corresponding to that point in time). (D) The sample with the maximum value after the weighting is chosen as the current percept. (E) A new sample at the 6th time instant is now in the window—the exponential decay factor is applied. (F) The new maximum probability sample is now the sample that arrived at the 6th second; this causes a switch in percepts. The last percept in memory was V A, now it has been changed to V B.
Figure 11
 
An illustration of the sampling procedure. (A) A bimodal posterior over viewpoint parameter ( x-axis). The two modes correspond to the two interpretations: V A percept and V B percept. (B) The probability values for samples chosen earlier in panel a shown: red = V A percept, green = V B percept. (C) 5 samples in window are shown. Each sample is generated at the point in time as indicated by time ( x-axis) axis. The red corresponds to the V A percept and the green corresponds to the V B percept. The exponential decay factor is applied to the samples (i.e., each sample's probability is weighted by the exponential decay factor's value corresponding to that point in time). (D) The sample with the maximum value after the weighting is chosen as the current percept. (E) A new sample at the 6th time instant is now in the window—the exponential decay factor is applied. (F) The new maximum probability sample is now the sample that arrived at the 6th second; this causes a switch in percepts. The last percept in memory was V A, now it has been changed to V B.
In (Schrater & Sundareswara, 2006), we analyze the probabilistic properties of this kind of sampling model in a more general setting and show that they generate Markov Renewal Processes. Note that memory update events (where samples in memory are exchanged) can be thought of as perceptual state transitions. These transitions can either be same-state or between state transitions, but we assume that same-state transitions are not perceptually salient. Additionally, we showed that MRP models can be good descriptions of our human bistability data. Here we illustrate the explanatory power of the theory described above by accounting for the main elements of our data with a simple implementation of a sampling-based decision model. 
Simulation methods
Posterior
A 2D image I of a Necker cube is theoretically is compatible with the projection of an infinite number of polyhedra, depending on the depth of its 8 vertices. However, human perception is dominated by two interpretations, both cuboidal in shape, that involve two different poses or viewpoints on the object. Therefore, we simplified the 8-dimensional Necker cube vertex—depth space into a 1D viewpoint (elevation) parameter space θ of the observer (we will henceforth refer to the viewpoint elevation parameter as just viewpoint parameter for brevity.) The viewpoint likelihood defined by P( Iθ) is modeled as the sum of two Gaussians centered on each of the two Necker cube interpretation modes, θ V A and θ V B. Assuming a Gaussian prior on viewpoint centered on θ M, the posterior, P( θI) is proportional to P( Iθ) P( θ). 
Memory decay
Memory decay is implemented by exponential function e t/ τ that discounts the weights attached to samples as a function of their age t. The rate of decay is determined by the parameter τ. The sampling rate was set to 2 samples/s. 
Sampling and perceptual decisions
The sampling is carried out as follows. At the start of the simulations, “ n” independent samples are drawn i.i.d. from the posterior distribution. The number “n” is set to 2 τ. The order in which we process the samples is indicative of the temporal nature of the sampling. The memory decay function ( e t/ τ) is multiplied by the posterior probability of the samples, where “ t” indexes the samples. The sample with the largest weight is selected as the current percept—the orientation of this sample represents the perceived orientation of the Necker cube. Subsequently, new samples are drawn in the same way and added to the current set of samples, and samples older than 2 τ seconds are discarded. 
Simulations
To illustrate model properties we varied two parameters in the simulations, the ratio of posterior heights at the peaks of the two likelihoods and the memory decay rate. These two parameters were varied because they corresponded to hypothesized differences between participants (this is confirmed by Figure 13). 
We varied the ratio of posterior heights to account for potential differences in response probabilities and cumulative transition probabilities between subjects. There are many ways to vary the posterior heights, but we were particularly interested in assessing the effect of varying the width of the prior distribution because the width of the prior distribution encodes how strongly viewpoint is biased by the context manipulation. Leaving the prior mean θ M fixed to reflect either context V A or V B, the relative heights of the two posterior peaks can be controlled by varying the prior variance σ 2, which adjusts the ratio of the prior probability values at the location of the two peaks of the likelihood:  
ω = P ( θ V A ) P ( θ V B ) = exp ( ( θ V B θ M ) 2 ( θ V A θ M ) 2 2 σ 2 ) ,
(8)
where θ V A is the mean parameter value that represents the “view from above” percept and θ V B is the mean that represents the “view from below” percept. 
We varied ω between 0.1 and 10 for each context condition. For simplicity, we rescaled the viewpoint axis so that the two Necker cube percepts corresponded to −1 ( V A percept at mode θ V A) and +1 ( V B percept at mode θ V B), and the likelihood standard deviations were set to 0.25. The mean of the prior, θ M, was fixed at 0.1 or at −0.1, depending on which context condition we were simulating. 
To model potential differences in memory discounting between observers, we varied a second simulation parameter, the exponential memory decay constant τ, which determines how quickly a sample's posterior probability decays. Assuming that 2 samples are generated every second, we varied the memory decay constant ( τ) from 20 s to 0.0198. 
Properties of the model
The sampling model generates a number of interesting properties that can be described by Markov Renewal Processes (MRP). MRPs are generated by distributions Q ij( t) = π ij F ij( t), which represent the probability that the process makes a transition to state j in time less than or equal to t, given the process just entered state i. MRPs transition between states via a Markov chain generated by the transition matrix π ij = P( s N = js N−1 = i). The times between state transitions are described by temporal distribution functions F ij( t) that depend on the current state and the next state entered. 
Sampling from this generating process involves picking an initial state, using the transition matrix to draw subsequent states, then sampling a duration from the appropriate temporal distribution. In (Schrater & Sundareswara, 2006), we derive the correspondence between the sampling model and MRPs. There we show the transition matrix parameters in πij are a function of the relative probability mass under the posterior peaks. Because the sampling we used was independent (i.e., the transition to a new state is not dependent on the previous state), the rows in the transition matrix of the corresponding MRP model are equal to each other. The MRP temporal distribution functions Fij correspond to how long a maximally weighted sample in the Bayesian model remains the maximum before replacement. 
In summary, in the Bayesian model, the ratio of the mass under the posterior peaks and the memory decay determine the amount of time spent in each percept; in the MRP model, this translates to the entries of the transition matrix ( π ij) and the probability of duration in each state given by entries of the temporal distribution matrix F ij
In the next section, we show that the model-generated MRP has temporal distributions F ij that are approximately exponential and the same for all four state transitions . In addition, because we used independent sampling, the transition probability is independent of the previous state: i.e., π ij = π jj. Thus, the MRP process for bistability reduces to just 2 parameters (the probability of transition to V A and mean of the exponential temporal function). 
We can use this correspondence to elucidate key properties of the sampling-based decision model. First, a percept's duration is the sum of “ k” sample times from the exponential distribution, where “ k” is the number of self-state transitions that occur before a state-change transition. Because means and variances of i.i.d. random variables sum, we predict correlation between means and variances of percept durations. Moreover, the probability of self-state transitions plays the critical role in the determining perceptual duration distributions. Each self-state transition contributes an exponentially distributed random time—if the number of self-state transitions was fixed rather than stochastic, percept durations would be gamma-distributed (the sum of “ k” exponentials is gamma-distributed with parameter λ has mean and k/ λ and variance k/ λ 2). Since k is random, the duration distributions are mixtures of exponentials, and the probability of self-state transition determines this mixture. Because the relative number of self-state transitions is determined by the posterior probabilities, the effects of changing the posterior probabilities will cause specific, predictable changes in the shape of the percept duration distributions, the survival probability functions, and the mean and variances of the percept durations. We test these predictions in the results section below. 
Simulation results
In this section, we compare the results of the simulations with our experimental data. To better compare human and model behavior, we simulated the experimental data collection procedure to collect model responses and analyzed the simulated response data the same way as human data. 
First we show that the simulated data exhibits switching distributions similar to humans. Figure 12 shows simulated cumulative transition and survival probability curves for the maximum-likelihood parameters that best-matched human data. Mean and confidence intervals were obtained by obtaining maximum likelihood estimates for 50 bootstrapped samples of human data. The mean maximum likelihood parameters for context V A are posterior peak ratio, ω = 1.1015 ± 0.0165 (1 SD) and τ = 0.0846 s = ±0.02 (1 SD). The mean estimate obtained is the 0.632+ estimate (Efron & Tibshirani, 1997). The mean maximum likelihood parameters for context VB are posterior peak ω = 0.9645 ± 0.0127 (1 SD) and τ = .0652 ± .01 (1 SD). Since the memory decay parameter, τ is similar within 1 standard deviation between contexts, it is not a parameter that explains the difference between context behavior. Instead the single parameter ω (which determines the heights of the posterior peak ratio) explains the difference in cumulative transition and survival probability functions induced by context. We also fit the mean durations of both the subject and the simulated data durations with gamma curves; all the fits returned could not be rejected. 
Figure 12
 
Comparison of pooled human (red) and model (blue) survival probability curves for the best-matched simulation parameters. Survival probability curves for (A) Percept VA in context VA. (B) VB in context VA. Simulations for context VA run with best-matched parameters:ω = 1.1015 ± .0165 (C) VA in context VB. (D) VB in context VB. Best-matched simulation parameters 1/ω = 1.037 ± 0.0127. Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap SE.
Figure 12
 
Comparison of pooled human (red) and model (blue) survival probability curves for the best-matched simulation parameters. Survival probability curves for (A) Percept VA in context VA. (B) VB in context VA. Simulations for context VA run with best-matched parameters:ω = 1.1015 ± .0165 (C) VA in context VB. (D) VB in context VB. Best-matched simulation parameters 1/ω = 1.037 ± 0.0127. Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap SE.
Next we addressed whether variations in simulation parameter can capture much of the between-subjects variation. In Figure 13 below, we plot phase duration means against variances for each of the 20 participant's data in the context condition. Plotted on the same graph are simulated means and variances, where each point corresponds to a different pair of posterior ratio (at the two peaks of the likelihood) and memory decay values. Despite the simplicity of the theoretical assumptions and simulation choices, the model produced bistable behavior similar to human data, and the variation in simulation parameters captures most of the spread of human data. Note that the simulated data follow the same “arc-like” trend of the human observers for the context consistent condition (the arc is a consequence of the data collection method—since we only collected responses for up to 10 s after the start of the block, this caused variability to decrease for mean durations greater than half the data collection period). Additionally, variation in simulation parameters generates correlations between means and variances for all four conditions that are comparable with human data. 
Figure 13
 
Comparisons of phase duration means vs. variance in context-consistent and context-inconsistent phases. In all Figures A–D, red points indicate subject data and blue points indicate simulated data for one pair of ( ω, τ) parameter values. (A) Context-consistent view-from-above data ( V A context/ V A percept). There is an arc-like pattern (which is a result of the data collection) in the human data that is mirrored by the simulated data. (B) Context-inconsistent V B percept data ( V B percept durations in V A context). (C) Context-inconsistent V A percept data. (D) Context-consistent V B percept data ( V B percept survival in V B context).
Figure 13
 
Comparisons of phase duration means vs. variance in context-consistent and context-inconsistent phases. In all Figures A–D, red points indicate subject data and blue points indicate simulated data for one pair of ( ω, τ) parameter values. (A) Context-consistent view-from-above data ( V A context/ V A percept). There is an arc-like pattern (which is a result of the data collection) in the human data that is mirrored by the simulated data. (B) Context-inconsistent V B percept data ( V B percept durations in V A context). (C) Context-inconsistent V A percept data. (D) Context-consistent V B percept data ( V B percept survival in V B context).
To better characterize the statistical properties of the simulation model, we computed the MRP generators that corresponded to the best-match simulation parameters for context V A. From the best-matching simulations, we computed π from the percentage of memory update events that were of each transition type. The resulting transition for V A context was:  
P i j = ( . 6415 . 3585 . 6415 . 3585 ) .
(9)
The first row of the matrix shows that 0 → 0 transitions ( π 00) occurred at almost twice the rate of 0 → 1 ( π 01), and similarly the second row shows 1 → 0 ( π 10) occurred at almost twice the rate of 1 → 1 transitions ( π 11). 
The resulting transition matrix for V B context was  
P i j = ( 0.4448 0.5552 0.4448 0.5552 ) .
(10)
The difference in transition probability between contexts captures all the effects of context on switching behavior in the model. 
As previously mentioned, sampling from this model generates temporal distribution functions that approximately exponential and the same for all transitions. 
The MRP generator temporal distribution functions F ij( t) for context V B are shown in Figure 14 These curves were obtained by computing histograms of the amount of time between memory updates. Notice that they are similar to exponential functions and to each other, implying that they share the same temporal distribution mean. The temporal distribution functions for V A context show similar behavior. 
Figure 14
 
Temporal distribution functions for all transitions recorded while simulating bistable data (best-matched human parameters for V B context were used for the simulation). The simulation gives rise to temporal distribution functions that are approximately exponential and are similar for all transitions.
Figure 14
 
Temporal distribution functions for all transitions recorded while simulating bistable data (best-matched human parameters for V B context were used for the simulation). The simulation gives rise to temporal distribution functions that are approximately exponential and are similar for all transitions.
In summary, a prior bias, which determines the posterior heights in the model, affects the probability of transitioning into V A and V B percepts, effectively by changing the probability of self-state transitions. The simplicity of this model is surprising given the alternatives. For example, adaptation or fatigue models of bistability make no clear predictions about the effect of context—the distribution of percept durations could be almost anything, and the distributions could change in many ways. 
Discussion of simulation
We presented a sampling-based decision-making model that modeled the effect of context on bistability. Using exponentially weighted samples from the posterior distribution, we were able to produce switching behavior similar to human observers. From the standpoint of MRP generators, the simulations generated a very simple model, and that model captures much of human switching dynamics. However, the sampling idea presented above can generate much more complicated behavior. In particular, in Schrater and Sundareswara (2006), we show that the independent sampling assumption can be relaxed, and that relaxing that assumption generates a richer class of MRP models. The simulations also rely on a fixed posterior distribution, which is unreasonable. A better model of the perceptual inference should incorporate the effects of eye movements, memory, and learning that cause the posterior distribution to vary with time. 
As an example of the simplifications in the probability model, we used a likelihood model on viewpoint (pose) P( Iθ). A more realistic representation would incorporate likelihood functions that implement perspective projection constraints in the inference of 3D objects from 2D, the effects of eye movements on the image data, and priors embodying the typical biases found in interpreting line drawings, like preference for symmetry between 3D edges, or compactness. Additionally, the current system could be embedded in a hierarchical recurrent inference system similar to the one proposed by Lee and Mumford (2003). However, the simple simulations were sufficient to illustrate the idea that implementing Bayesian decision making via sampling can create human-like switching behavior. 
In the basic implementation presented here, we assume that switching behavior is generated by a stationary process. However, a non-stationary switching process is easy to simulate, although harder to characterize mathematically. By allowing posterior probabilities to vary with time (e.g., due to eye fixations or previous stimuli), we believe many reported bistability effects could be accounted for by models that compute Bayesian decisions via sampling (see Discussion). 
Our approach is related to a model proposed by Gepshtein and Kubovy (2005) to account for data from experiments with bistable dot-lattices. The authors posit that bistability in successive presentation of dot lattices results from an intrinsic bias that stochastically drifts in orientation space, causing a change in perception. Their “intrinsic bias” and “stimulus support” are similar to prior and likelihood, respectively. Percepts are probabilistically generated at transition times, where the probability of a percept depends on the ratio of its intrinsic bias and stimulus support. Their model can be viewed as an instance of a sampling model, but with no memory and a non-stationary prior distribution (the drifting intrinsic bias). In fact, the key to generating switching behavior in their model is the drift in the “intrinsic bias.” In contrast, the most important aspects of the model used in our simulation are self-state transitions. Both models show the potential explanatory power of sampling within an inference framework to account for bistability data. 
Discussion
When a bistable image was embedded in a context that favored one of the interpretations, perceptual ambiguity was reduced; the interpretation supported by the context persists longer and occurs more frequently. Additionally, the duration of the percept that is not supported by the context is shortened relative to baseline (without any context). These empirical results suggest context is modulating the perceptual ambiguity of the Necker cube in a manner consonant with Bayesian models of perceptual inference. Our model's successful prediction of both the time course of perceptual switching and the impact of context suggest that perceptual bistability may occur as a by-product of normal perceptual decision making: postulating special mechanisms for switching may be unnecessary. In particular, the same processes may mediate lengthening and shortening of duration of bistable percepts (a view also suggested by Gepshtein & Kubovy, 2005). This view is not supported by binocular rivalry results, where supporting context might prolong the consistent percept but does not affect the inconsistent percept (Sobel & Blake, 2002). 
In this paper, we introduced a perceptual decision-making model based on Bayesian inference as an approach to modeling bistability. The model is based on searching for the high probability interpretation of the Necker cube by sampling a posterior probability distribution on interpretation parameters over time, together with exponential discounting of past samples. We showed that this model is capable of producing bistable-like behavior when the posterior distribution is bimodal. Much of the explanatory power of this model stems from its reliance on Bayesian models of perceptual inference. Bayesian models use likelihood terms to encode knowledge about image rendering by expressing the scene parameters consistent with the image. For the Necker cube, a likelihood would express that 3D vertex depths must be consistent with the image lines and vertices. When combined with a prior that promotes 3D interpretations with equal angles between edges, the posterior distribution is bimodal on pose or viewpoint. Within this framework, the effects of context can be simply described as influencing prior probabilities of scene interpretations. 
Can other effects of perceptual multi-stability be explained within our theoretical framework? We believe the theory provides a natural interface to interpret both high-level and low-level effects on bistability because any event that has an impact on the relative probability of perceptual interpretations can influence decision making in the model. In particular, manipulations of variables that produce changes in a Bayesian interpretation of the ambiguous image should impact perceptual bistability. For example, variables that influence perceptual stability like eye position (Einhäuser, Martin, & König, 2004; Ellis & Stark, 1978; Kawabata, Yamagami, & Noaki, 1978) and attention (Hol et al., 2003; Meng & Tong, 2004; Toppino, 2003; van Ee et al., 2006; van Ee et al., 2005) are known to affect perceptual decision making as well. To explain these effects with our model, we would need to show that these variables mediate their influence by changing the relative probability of perceptual interpretations. 
The idea that perceptual multi-stability originates at a stage of visual processing involving perceptual decision making may help reconcile perceptual multi- stability and binocular rivalry. We showed that context could be modeled as influencing a prior distribution on interpretations for bistability. The strong similarities between rivalry and bistability effects raises the possibility that rivalry may result from a similar kind of perceptual decision process as bistability, except that image interpretations choices in rivalry are coupled with choices about data sources. 
Quantifying perceptual bistability
The measurement framework introduced in this paper can be used to address several complex issues that arise in the interpretation of bistability data, including stationarity, covariation between phase duration measures such as mean and variance, and relationships between state-transition models and percept duration distributions. 
Intuitively, stationarity measures whether the probability distributions governing perceptual state transitions vary across time. A stochastic process with stationary dynamics will converge to a steady-state distribution after a burn-in period and maintain that distribution for all subsequent time. Convergence occurs when the probability of observing a state is independent of previous states after sufficient elapsed time T. Moreover, the rate of convergence conveys important information about the strength of dependence between subsequent states. By definition, survival probability functions capture exactly the information needed to assess both stationarity and the rate of convergence. For percept V B, stationarity implies there is an elapsed time T such that the probability of the present state is independent of the past:  
P ( V B ( t + T ) | V B ( t ) ) = P ( V B ( t + T ) | V A ( t ) ) .
(11)
Over a finite data collection interval, a test of this condition will indicate whether the process is non-stationary (or failed to converge in the data collection period if the required T is longer). This condition is the same as requiring that survival probability asymptotes agree, which follows by expressing the right hand side of Equation 11 in terms of a survival probability function. By laws of conditional probability,  
P ( V B ( T + τ ) | V A ( τ ) ) = 1 P ( V A ( T + τ ) | V A ( τ ) ) .
(12)
Plugging in Equation 11 yields a testable condition for stationarity: there must exist a time T such that survival probabilities agree according to the equation:  
P ( V B ( T + τ ) | V B ( τ ) ) = 1 P ( V A ( T + τ ) | V A ( τ ) ) .
(13)
For times greater than T, the survival probability functions should converge to the response probabilities, if the process is stationary. In contrast, there exist non-stationary processes (e.g., periodic processes) that can express long-range dependence between states and will not pass this test. 
We present the survival probability curves from our baseline and context experiment re-plotted in Figure 15. In both figures, the dark curves shows P( V A( t + τ)∣ V A( τ)) and the light curves shows 1 − P( V B( t + τ)∣ V B( τ)). Both context and baseline data appear stationary and converge within the data collection period, within measurement error. The rapid convergence observed in our Necker cube data contrasts with the results of Mamassian and Goutcher (2005) for binocular rivalry data. Using survival probability functions, they found rivalry states were strongly dependent on the initial state, and this dependence persisted for at least an order of magnitude longer than we found in our data. 
Figure 15
 
Assessing stationarity using survival probability curves. (A) No-context condition: Because the dark-blue (VA survival) has the same asymptote as the light-blue curve (1 − VB survival), the survival of a percept at asymptote is independent of initial state-indicating a stationary process (B) Similar behavior is observed in the case of context. The two red survival curves correspond to VA context and the two green curves to VB context. 1 SE bars are shown.
Figure 15
 
Assessing stationarity using survival probability curves. (A) No-context condition: Because the dark-blue (VA survival) has the same asymptote as the light-blue curve (1 − VB survival), the survival of a percept at asymptote is independent of initial state-indicating a stationary process (B) Similar behavior is observed in the case of context. The two red survival curves correspond to VA context and the two green curves to VB context. 1 SE bars are shown.
The covariation between means and variances we observed across participants has been found in several other studies (Borsellino, De Marco, Allazetta, Rinesi, & Bartolini, 1972; Mamassian & Goutcher, 2005). We show that our theoretical model generates an MRP with simple properties and covariation that strongly resembles our participant's data. Thus, our approach provides a compelling explanation for one of the recurring patterns in bistability data. 
Traditionally, bistability data have been characterized by measuring phase duration distributions, frequently using gamma distributions. Markov Renewal Processes employing same-state transitions and exponentially distributed times between state transition events produce phase durations that can be well-approximated by Gamma distributions, even though not exactly Gamma. Rather than providing a better fitting family of distributions, the introduction of Markov Renewal Processes provides a mathematical framework for expressing properties and relationships between model properties and measures of the dynamics of bistable behavior of the visual system. 
Conclusions
In this paper, we showed how perceptual multi-stability could be interpreted as arising from a general process of probabilistic decision making with ambiguous images, without requiring special mechanisms to implement switching via adaptation or hysteresis. 
In the model, a decision is made as to which percept is currently experienced by sampling from a probability distribution across possible interpretations, and percepts are selected as samples with the largest time-discounted probability. We show that the model can reproduce the detailed dynamics of bistability, including covariation between means and variances of percept durations and the inverse relationship between the durations of the two percepts. We find that introducing regularity in the orientations of background objects influences the dynamics of bistability in ways quantitatively fitted by the model. To characterize the temporal dynamics effectively, we used Markov Renewal Processes; this quantification both allows the process to be described mathematically with its measures and allows us to introduce the same-state transitions that underlie the success of our modeling approach. 
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary File - Supplementary File 
Supplementary Figure - Supplementary Figure 
Figure 1. Figure illustrates phase durations produces by MRP with exponential generators and same-state transitions. Many values of the generator parameters produce phase duration densities well approximated by a gamma distribution. An example (green curve) is shown superimposed with a gamma curve with matching mean and variance (red curve). 
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by a grant by National Institute of Health (NIH-EY015261-01). We would like to thank Chris Kallie for discussions on experimental design. We are also very grateful to our reviewers for their thorough and insightful reviews. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Rashmi Sundareswara. 
Email: sundares@cs.umn.edu. 
Address: EECS 200 Union Street SE, Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA. 
References
Alais, D. Blake, R. (1971). Grouping visual features during binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 39, 4341–4353. [PubMed] [CrossRef]
Attneave, F. (1971). Multistability in perception. Scientific American, 225, 63–71. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Blake, R. (1989). A neural theory of binocular rivalry. Psychological Review, 96, 145–167. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Blake, R. Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 3, 13–21. [PubMed] [CrossRef]
Borsellino, A. De Marco, A. Allazetta, A. Rinesi, S. Bartolini, B. (1972). Reversal time distribution in the perception of visual ambiguous stimuli. Kybernetik, 10, 139–144. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Davis, J. Schiffman, H. R. Greist-Bousquet, S. (1990). Semantic context and figure-ground organization. Psychological Research, 52, 306–309. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dawson, M. R. (1987). Moving contexts do affect the perceived direction of apparent motion in motion competition displays. Vision Research, 27, 799–809. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Efron, B. Tibshirani, R. J. (1993). An introduction to the bootstrap.. New York: Chapman & Hall.
Efron, B. Tibshirani, R. J. (1997). Improvements on cross validation: The 632+ Bootstrap method. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 92, 548–560.
Einhäuser, W. Martin, K. A. König, P. (2004). Are switches in perception of the Necker cube related to eye position? European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 2811–2818. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ellis, S. R. Stark, L. (1978). Eye movements during the viewing of Necker cubes. Perception, 7, 575–581. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gepshtein, S. Kubovy, M. (2005). Stability and change in perception: Spatial organization in temporal context. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 487–495. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hol, K. Koene, A. van Ee, R. (2003). Attention-biased multi-stable surface perception in three-dimensional structure-from-motion. Journal of Vision, 3, (7):3, 486–498, http://journalofvision.org/3/7/3/, doi:10.1167/3.7.3. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Kawabata, N. Yamagami, K. Noaki, M. (1978). Visual fixation points and depth perception. Vision Research, 18, 853–854. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kersten, D. Mamassian, P. Yuille, A. (2004). Object perception as Bayesian inference. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 271–304. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kersten, D. Schrater, P. R. Mausfeld, R. Heyer, D. (2002). Pattern inference theory: A probabilistic approach to vision. Perception and the physical world. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Kramer, P. Yantis, S. (1997). Perceptual grouping in space and time: Evidence from the Ternus display. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 87–99. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lee, T. S. Mumford, D. (2003). Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 20, 1434–1448. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lehky, S. R. (1988). An astable multivibrator model of binocular rivalry. Perception, 17, 215–228. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Leopold, D. A. Logothetis, N. K. (1999). Multistable phenomena: Changing views in perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 254–264. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Long, G. M. Toppino, T. C. Mondin, G. W. (1992). Prime time: Fatigue and set effects in the perception of reversible figures. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 609–616. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mamassian, P. Goutcher, R. (2005). Temporal dynamics in bistable perception. Journal of Vision, 5, (4):7, 361–375, http://journalofvision.org/5/4/7/, doi:10.1167/5.4.7. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Mamassian, P. Landy, M. S. (1998). Observer biases in the 3D interpretation of line drawings. Vision Research, 38, 2817–2832, http://journalofvision.org/4/7/2/, doi:10.1167/4.7.2. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Meng, M. Tong, F. (2004). Can attention selectively bias bistable perception Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision, 4, (7):2, 539–551, http://journalofvision.org/4/7/2/, doi:10.1167/4.7.2. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Pelton, L. H. Solley, C. M. (1968). Acceleration of reversals of a Necker cube. American Journal of Psychology, 81, 585–588. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Peterson, M. A. Gibson, B. S. (1994). Object recognition contributions to figure-ground organizations: Operations on outlines and subjective contours. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 551–564. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Peterson, M. A. Harvey, E. M. Weidenbacher, H. J. (1991). Shape recognition contributions to figure-ground reversal: Which route counts? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 1075–1089. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pouget, A. Dayan, P. Zemel, R. S. (2003). Inference and computation with population codes. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 26, 381–410. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rock, I. Hall, S. Davis, J. (1994). Why do ambiguous figures reverse? Acta Psychologica, 87, 33–59. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ross, S. (1992). Applied probability models with optimization applications.. Mineola, NY: Dover Press.
Schrater, P. R. Sundareswara, R. (2006). Theory and dynamics of perceptual bistability. Neural Information Processing Systems, 19, 1217–1224.
Sobel, K. V. Blake, R. (2002). How context influences predominance during binocular rivalry. Perception, 31, 813–824. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Taylor, M. M. Aldridge, K. D. (1974). Stochastic processes in reversing figure perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 9–27. [CrossRef]
Toppino, T. C. (2003). Reversible-figure perception: Mechanisms of intentional control. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 1285–1295. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
van Ee, R. Adams, W. J. Mamassian, P. (2003). Bayesian modeling of cue interaction: Bistability in stereoscopic slant perception. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 20, 1398–1406. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
van Ee, R. Noest, A. J. Brascamp, J. W. van den Berg, A. V. (2006). Attentional control over either of the two competing percepts of ambiguous stimuli revealed by a two-parameter analysis: Means do not make the difference. Vision Research, 46, 3129–3141. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
van Ee, R. van Dam, L. C. Brouwer, G. J. (2005). Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability. Vision Research, 45, 41–55. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Walker, P. (1978). Binocular rivalry: Central or peripheral selective processes? Psychological Bulletin, 85, 376–389. [CrossRef]
Yuille, A. Kersten, D. (1978). Vision as Bayesian inference: Analysis by synthesis?? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 301–308. [CrossRef]
Figure 1
 
Necker stimulus used in the experiment. Participants fixated the central green dot and reported their perceptual state via key-press when cued by the sound of a beep.
Figure 1
 
Necker stimulus used in the experiment. Participants fixated the central green dot and reported their perceptual state via key-press when cued by the sound of a beep.
Figure 2
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V A orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
Figure 2
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V A orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
Figure 3
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V B orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
Figure 3
 
An example of an orientation discrimination task for unambiguously oriented cubes in V B orientation. Subjects are asked to indicate which column was more fronto-parallel.
Figure 4
 
An instance of the V A context stimulus.
Figure 4
 
An instance of the V A context stimulus.
Figure 5
 
An instance of V B context stimulus.
Figure 5
 
An instance of V B context stimulus.
Figure 6
 
P R’ measure: Response of V B percept as a function of time. The blue curve represents baseline data (no context), the red curve data are P R when the context shown was V A, and green represents P Rwhen the context was V B context. The figure shows large initial response biases both with and without context. View from above context can be described as producing the same initial bias as baseline, but preserves more of the initial bias to asymptote around 0.3. V B context produces a strong bias toward V B responses that decreases to asymptote around 0.6 after 5 s. Dashed lines represented ±1 bootstrap SE of the estimate.
Figure 6
 
P R’ measure: Response of V B percept as a function of time. The blue curve represents baseline data (no context), the red curve data are P R when the context shown was V A, and green represents P Rwhen the context was V B context. The figure shows large initial response biases both with and without context. View from above context can be described as producing the same initial bias as baseline, but preserves more of the initial bias to asymptote around 0.3. V B context produces a strong bias toward V B responses that decreases to asymptote around 0.6 after 5 s. Dashed lines represented ±1 bootstrap SE of the estimate.
Figure 7
 
Average effect of context on cumulative transition probability conditioned on the initial perceptual state. The x-axis indicates time during a block and the y-axis is probability of having switched within the time indicated on the axis. Shows P T curves, given the initial percept is (A) view from above, V A, and (B) view from below, V B. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on cumulative transition probability (red = V A and green = V B, dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error) compared to no context (blue curves). Durations (time before transition) spent in the V A initial state (Figure A) are prolonged by V A context and shortened by V B context. However, while initial V B durations (Figure B) are prolonged by consistent context, there is little suppression of V B percept from view-from-above context when compared to the no-context (base-rate) condition. Note that context effects on distribution functions involve simultaneous shape and scale changes, corresponding to correlated changes in the phase duration means and variances.
Figure 7
 
Average effect of context on cumulative transition probability conditioned on the initial perceptual state. The x-axis indicates time during a block and the y-axis is probability of having switched within the time indicated on the axis. Shows P T curves, given the initial percept is (A) view from above, V A, and (B) view from below, V B. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on cumulative transition probability (red = V A and green = V B, dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error) compared to no context (blue curves). Durations (time before transition) spent in the V A initial state (Figure A) are prolonged by V A context and shortened by V B context. However, while initial V B durations (Figure B) are prolonged by consistent context, there is little suppression of V B percept from view-from-above context when compared to the no-context (base-rate) condition. Note that context effects on distribution functions involve simultaneous shape and scale changes, corresponding to correlated changes in the phase duration means and variances.
Figure 8
 
Effects of context on survival probabilities ( P S). (A) survival of V A initial percept shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. (B) survival of V B initial percepts shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on survival probability functions (red = V A and green = V B) compared to no context (blue curves). Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error. Note that all survival curves have the same asymptote as the response probability functions (which they should if MRPs provide a sufficient data model) shown in Figure 6.
Figure 8
 
Effects of context on survival probabilities ( P S). (A) survival of V A initial percept shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. (B) survival of V B initial percepts shown on the y-axis against time during a block shown on the x-axis. Context has both positive (prolonging) and negative (curtailing) effects on survival probability functions (red = V A and green = V B) compared to no context (blue curves). Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap standard error. Note that all survival curves have the same asymptote as the response probability functions (which they should if MRPs provide a sufficient data model) shown in Figure 6.
Figure 9
 
Effect of consistent and inconsistent context on mean percept durations. Mean percept durations with context are plotted against mean durations in the baseline condition, for each combination of percept and context type. Error bars represent 99% bootstrap confidence intervals. The blue line represents the values predicted if context had no effect. Points above the line correspond to percept durations increased by context (prolonging), while points below the line indicate context-induced shortening.
Figure 9
 
Effect of consistent and inconsistent context on mean percept durations. Mean percept durations with context are plotted against mean durations in the baseline condition, for each combination of percept and context type. Error bars represent 99% bootstrap confidence intervals. The blue line represents the values predicted if context had no effect. Points above the line correspond to percept durations increased by context (prolonging), while points below the line indicate context-induced shortening.
Figure 10
 
An illustration of the probability densities used in the model. The interpretation ambiguity is represented by a bi-modal density function on cube orientation θ (blue curve). The prior P(θ) encodes biases toward one of the interpretations, including the effects of context (green curve). The product of the likelihood and the prior produces the (normalized) posterior probability (red curve), from which we sample in our simulations.
Figure 10
 
An illustration of the probability densities used in the model. The interpretation ambiguity is represented by a bi-modal density function on cube orientation θ (blue curve). The prior P(θ) encodes biases toward one of the interpretations, including the effects of context (green curve). The product of the likelihood and the prior produces the (normalized) posterior probability (red curve), from which we sample in our simulations.
Figure 11
 
An illustration of the sampling procedure. (A) A bimodal posterior over viewpoint parameter ( x-axis). The two modes correspond to the two interpretations: V A percept and V B percept. (B) The probability values for samples chosen earlier in panel a shown: red = V A percept, green = V B percept. (C) 5 samples in window are shown. Each sample is generated at the point in time as indicated by time ( x-axis) axis. The red corresponds to the V A percept and the green corresponds to the V B percept. The exponential decay factor is applied to the samples (i.e., each sample's probability is weighted by the exponential decay factor's value corresponding to that point in time). (D) The sample with the maximum value after the weighting is chosen as the current percept. (E) A new sample at the 6th time instant is now in the window—the exponential decay factor is applied. (F) The new maximum probability sample is now the sample that arrived at the 6th second; this causes a switch in percepts. The last percept in memory was V A, now it has been changed to V B.
Figure 11
 
An illustration of the sampling procedure. (A) A bimodal posterior over viewpoint parameter ( x-axis). The two modes correspond to the two interpretations: V A percept and V B percept. (B) The probability values for samples chosen earlier in panel a shown: red = V A percept, green = V B percept. (C) 5 samples in window are shown. Each sample is generated at the point in time as indicated by time ( x-axis) axis. The red corresponds to the V A percept and the green corresponds to the V B percept. The exponential decay factor is applied to the samples (i.e., each sample's probability is weighted by the exponential decay factor's value corresponding to that point in time). (D) The sample with the maximum value after the weighting is chosen as the current percept. (E) A new sample at the 6th time instant is now in the window—the exponential decay factor is applied. (F) The new maximum probability sample is now the sample that arrived at the 6th second; this causes a switch in percepts. The last percept in memory was V A, now it has been changed to V B.
Figure 12
 
Comparison of pooled human (red) and model (blue) survival probability curves for the best-matched simulation parameters. Survival probability curves for (A) Percept VA in context VA. (B) VB in context VA. Simulations for context VA run with best-matched parameters:ω = 1.1015 ± .0165 (C) VA in context VB. (D) VB in context VB. Best-matched simulation parameters 1/ω = 1.037 ± 0.0127. Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap SE.
Figure 12
 
Comparison of pooled human (red) and model (blue) survival probability curves for the best-matched simulation parameters. Survival probability curves for (A) Percept VA in context VA. (B) VB in context VA. Simulations for context VA run with best-matched parameters:ω = 1.1015 ± .0165 (C) VA in context VB. (D) VB in context VB. Best-matched simulation parameters 1/ω = 1.037 ± 0.0127. Dashed lines represent ±1 bootstrap SE.
Figure 13
 
Comparisons of phase duration means vs. variance in context-consistent and context-inconsistent phases. In all Figures A–D, red points indicate subject data and blue points indicate simulated data for one pair of ( ω, τ) parameter values. (A) Context-consistent view-from-above data ( V A context/ V A percept). There is an arc-like pattern (which is a result of the data collection) in the human data that is mirrored by the simulated data. (B) Context-inconsistent V B percept data ( V B percept durations in V A context). (C) Context-inconsistent V A percept data. (D) Context-consistent V B percept data ( V B percept survival in V B context).
Figure 13
 
Comparisons of phase duration means vs. variance in context-consistent and context-inconsistent phases. In all Figures A–D, red points indicate subject data and blue points indicate simulated data for one pair of ( ω, τ) parameter values. (A) Context-consistent view-from-above data ( V A context/ V A percept). There is an arc-like pattern (which is a result of the data collection) in the human data that is mirrored by the simulated data. (B) Context-inconsistent V B percept data ( V B percept durations in V A context). (C) Context-inconsistent V A percept data. (D) Context-consistent V B percept data ( V B percept survival in V B context).
Figure 14
 
Temporal distribution functions for all transitions recorded while simulating bistable data (best-matched human parameters for V B context were used for the simulation). The simulation gives rise to temporal distribution functions that are approximately exponential and are similar for all transitions.
Figure 14
 
Temporal distribution functions for all transitions recorded while simulating bistable data (best-matched human parameters for V B context were used for the simulation). The simulation gives rise to temporal distribution functions that are approximately exponential and are similar for all transitions.
Figure 15
 
Assessing stationarity using survival probability curves. (A) No-context condition: Because the dark-blue (VA survival) has the same asymptote as the light-blue curve (1 − VB survival), the survival of a percept at asymptote is independent of initial state-indicating a stationary process (B) Similar behavior is observed in the case of context. The two red survival curves correspond to VA context and the two green curves to VB context. 1 SE bars are shown.
Figure 15
 
Assessing stationarity using survival probability curves. (A) No-context condition: Because the dark-blue (VA survival) has the same asymptote as the light-blue curve (1 − VB survival), the survival of a percept at asymptote is independent of initial state-indicating a stationary process (B) Similar behavior is observed in the case of context. The two red survival curves correspond to VA context and the two green curves to VB context. 1 SE bars are shown.
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary Figure
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×