Figure 3 shows the results obtained for the second-order condition. We found a pattern of results very similar to that found with first-order stimuli: a repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect of the adaptation [
F(3,7) = 15.48,
p < 0.01], a significant main effect of the ISI [
F(5,7) = 8.56,
p < 0.01], and a significant interaction between adaptation and ISI [
F(15,7) = 5.08,
p < 0.01]. Pairwise comparisons revealed no significant differences between adaptation at 80 ms and 160 ms (
p > 0.05) for ISI of 40 ms, whereas we found significant differences between the adaptation at 80 ms and the adaptation at 320 (
p = 0.007), and between adaptation at 80 ms and 640 ms (
p < 0.05), both for ISIs of 40 ms. In addition, we found a significant difference between the adaptation at 160 ms and the adaptation at 640 ms for ISIs of 40 ms. We also found significant differences between the adaptation at 80 ms and the other adaptation durations for the 120 ms ISI (
p < 0.05), but no other significant differences between the other adaptation durations for the same ISI, nor for ISIs longer than 120 ms. Also for second-order stimuli we conducted one-sample
t-tests in order to assess if a particular combination of adaptation and ISI gives a significant bias either towards the same or different direction with respect to the adaptation pattern. Adapting for 80 ms to a second-order stimulus and presenting an ambiguous second-order test pattern after an ISI of 40 ms produced a significant bias towards the direction of the adaptation pattern [
t(7) = 3.28,
p < 0.05] (73% of responses in the same direction) (rVMP). Moreover, we obtained a significant bias towards the adapting direction at 80 ms with an ISI of 2 seconds [
t(7) = 3.27,
p < 0.05] even though the percentage of responses in the same direction is quite low (63% of same responses) (PS). Similarly, adapting for 160 ms biases the perceived direction of the test pattern towards the direction of the adapting stimulus, but only with an ISI of 5 s [
t(7) = 3.31,
p < 0.05], even if the magnitude of the effect was quite small (63% of same responses) (PS). Longer adaptation durations biased the perceived direction of ambiguous second-order patterns to the opposite direction of the adapting stimulus resulting in rMAE. In particular, adapting for 320 ms biased the perceived position in the opposite direction across the shorter ISI durations; that is, 40 ms (25% of same responses) [
t(7) = −3.96,
p = 0.005], 120 ms (21% of same responses) [
t(7) = −5.49,
p = 0.01] and 480 ms (35% of same responses) [
t(7) = −3.24,
p < 0.05]. Similar results were obtained adapting at 640 ms. Indeed, we found significant biases opposite the direction of the adapting pattern only for the shorter ISIs: 40 ms (17% of same responses) [
t(7) = −4.77,
p = 0.002] and 120 ms (21% of same responses) [
t(7) = −5.40,
p = 0.001]. These multiple one-sample
t-tests pointed out slightly different time courses about rVMP, rMAE and PS for first- and second-order motion. However, it should be noticed that we did not obtain a significant effect of the motion order (either as a main effect or as interaction), suggesting that the differences obtained between first- and second-order motion are indeed very small.