However, several studies have shown that perceptual learning can escape roving disruption when the roving stimuli are sufficiently different (Tartaglia, Aberg, & Herzog,
2009; Zhang et al.,
2008) or are presented in different retinal locations (Otto, Herzog, Fahle, & Zhaoping,
2006), probably to reduce the overlap of corresponding neural populations. Long-lasting training is also shown to help learning under stimulus roving (Parkosadze, Otto, Malania, Kezeli, & Herzog,
2008). Specific to the current study, our previous study also shows that learning can escape roving disruption when each roving stimulus is given a letter tag (Zhang et al.,
2008). The observers are unable to learn the discrimination of four roving contrasts (i.e., 0.20, 0.30, 0.47, and 0.63). But significant learning becomes evident when a distinct letter tag (i.e., A, B, C, or D) is assigned to each contrast and is shown before a relevant trial. Additional evidence shows that contrast thresholds are unaffected by stimulus sequencing or roving at the beginning of training (Adini et al.,
2004; Zhang et al.,
2008) or after learning is established through sequenced stimulus presentations (Kuai et al.,
2005). Therefore, it is not the encoding and retrieval stages, but the consolidation stage, of information processing that are more affected by contrast roving. We suggest that the letter tags may help the brain to attend to the responses of proper sets of neurons for learning consolidation (Zhang et al.,
2008).